Advertisement

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

, Volume 37, Issue 3, pp 553–563 | Cite as

Emotional Responsivity in Children with Autism, Children with Other Developmental Disabilities, and Children with Typical Development

  • D. J. Scambler
  • S. Hepburn
  • M. D. Rutherford
  • E. A. Wehner
  • S. J. Rogers
Original Paper

Abstract

Twenty six children with autism, 24 children with developmental disabilities, and 15 typically developing children participated in tasks in which an adult displayed emotions. Child focus of attention, change in facial tone (i.e., hedonic tone), and latency to changes in tone were measured and summary scores of emotional contagion were created. Group differences existed in the ratio of episodes that resulted in emotional contagion. Correlations existed between measures of emotional contagion, measures of joint attention, and indices of severity of autism. Children with autism demonstrated muted changes in affect, but these responses occurred much less frequently than in comparison groups. The findings suggest directions for early identification and early treatment of autism.

Keywords

Young children with autism Developmental disabilities Empathy Emotional responsiveness 

Notes

Acknowledgments

First Author was supported by an institutional NRSA Postdoctoral Research Training Program Grant, “Development of Maladaptive Behavior,” # NIMH 5 T32 MH15442-24 and a young researcher’s award from the Developmental Psychobiology Research Group (DPRG). This research was funded by grants from the NICHD, #PO1/U19 HD035468, to Fifth Author. It is part of the NICHD/NIDCD Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism (CPEA). Third Author was supported by NIH grant 2T32MH15780-20. Fifth Author was also supported by Maternal and Child Health Bureau Grant # 2T73MC00011-04, and by the Administration on Developmental Disabilities Grant # 90DD0414. Thanks to Angela John, Athena Hayes, and Jennifer Stella for their help with coding and to the children and families for their participation.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual (4th ed.). Washington, D.C.: APA.Google Scholar
  2. Bacon, A. L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., & Allen, D. (1998). The responses of autistic children to the distress of others. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 129–142.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Charman, T. (2004). Matching preschool children with autism spectrum disorders and comparison children for language ability: Methodological challenges. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 65–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Arbelle, S., Wellington, P., & Sigman, M. (1998). Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioral and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. Child Development, 69, 1494–1502.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., & Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 335–345.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dawson, G., & Lewy, A. L. (1989). Arousal, attention, and the socioemotional impairments in individuals with autism. In G. Dawson (Ed.) Autism: Nature, diagnosis, & treatment (pp. 49–74). New York NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J. Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Emde, R. (1983). The prerepresentational self and its affective core. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 38, 165–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1998). The child with special needs: encouraging intellectual and emotional growth. Reading, MA: Perseus Books.Google Scholar
  11. Gutstein, S. E., & Sheely, R. K. (2002). Relationship development intervention with young children: Social and emotional development activities for Asperger Syndrome, autism, PPD and NLD. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
  12. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 40, 649–659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Joseph, R. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1997). An investigation of attention and affect in children with autism and down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 385–396.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2, 320–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lord, C. (1995). Follow-up of two-year-olds referred for possible autism. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 36, 1365–1382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview – revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lord, C., Risi, A., DiLavore, P., & Rutter, M., (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G). Los Angeles, CA: The Western Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  19. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & DiLavore, P. (1997). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G). Washington, DC: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  20. Loveland, K. A. (2001). Toward an ecological theory of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman et al (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 17–37). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  21. Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning. Cranston, RI: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc.Google Scholar
  22. Mundy, P., Hogan, A., & Doehring, P. (1996). A preliminary manual for the abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami.Google Scholar
  23. Repacholi B. M., & Gopnick, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. Nadel, G. Butterworth et al. (Eds.) Imitation in Infancy: Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 254–283). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Rogers, S. J., Hall, T., Osaki, D., Reaven, J., & Herbison, J. (2001). The Denver model: A comprehensive, integrated educational approach to young children with autism and their families. In Handleman, J. S., & S. L. Harris (Eds.), Preschool Education Programs for Children with Autism (2nd ed., pp. 95–134). Austin, Texas: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
  26. Seibert, J., Hogan, A., & Mundy, P. (1982). Assessing social interactional competencies: The Early Social–Communication Scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244–258.Google Scholar
  27. Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J. H., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., Shapiro, T. (1987). Expression of emotion in young autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836–838.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sparrow S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  30. Stone, W. L., Lee, E. B., Ashford, L., Brissie, J., Hepburn, S. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Weiss, B. H. (1999). Can autism be diagnosed accurately in children under 3 years? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 40, 219–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the human infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  32. Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 725–735.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of empathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107–130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  • D. J. Scambler
    • 1
  • S. Hepburn
    • 2
  • M. D. Rutherford
    • 3
  • E. A. Wehner
    • 2
  • S. J. Rogers
    • 4
  1. 1.Oklahoma State UniversityStillwaterUSA
  2. 2.University of Colorado Health Sciences CenterDenverUSA
  3. 3.McMasters UniversityHamiltonCanada
  4. 4.The M.I.N.D. Institute, University of California DavisDavisUSA

Personalised recommendations