Skip to main content
Log in

Affective Prosody Perception and the Relation to Social Competence in Autistic and Typically Developing Children

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 14 December 2023

This article has been updated

Abstract

Individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have difficulty perceiving and expressing emotions. Since prosodic changes in speech (i.e. changes in intonation, stress, rhythm, etc.) are crucial for extracting information about the emotional state of a speaker, an inability to perceive and interpret these prosodic changes may be related to impairments in social communication. This study used non-verbal emotional voice-clips to examine the ability of autistic and typically-developing children (7–13 years old) to extract affect from changes in prosody. This research also explored whether difficulty extracting affective intent from changes in prosody may be related to social competence. Autistic (n = 26) and typically-developing (n = 26) children accurately matched emotional voice-clips to emotion words, suggesting autistic children can accurately extract the affective meaning conveyed by changes in prosody. Autistic children were less accurate at matching the voice-clips to emotional faces, suggesting that autistic children may struggle to make use of prosodic information in a social context. Across both autistic and typically-developing children, prosody-face matching accuracy was found to predict overall social competence, as well as social inferencing abilities, suggesting that the inability to utilize affective information derived from a speaker’s voice may interfere with effective social communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

Notes

  1. While person-first language such as “individuals with autism” is often the preference of researchers and clinicians, autistic individuals have indicated a preference for identity-first language, as it incorporates autism as a component of their identity over person-first language (61% versus 28%) (Kenny et al. 2016). A similar preference has also been expressed by parents of autistic children (51% versus 22%) (Kenny et al. 2016) and self-advocates (Sinclair 2013). As such, we will respect this preference and use identity-first language throughout this manuscript.

References

  • Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Allison, C. (2008). The autism spectrum quotient: Children’s version (AQ-Child). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1230–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balconi, M., & Carrera, A. (2007). Emotional representation in facial expression and script: A comparison between normal and autistic children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(4), 409–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R. A., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S. (2006). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ)—adolescent version. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bebko, J. M., Weiss, J. A., Demark, J. L., & Gomez, P. (2006). Discrimination of temporal synchrony in intermodal events by children with autism and children with developmental disabilities without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(1), 88–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belin, P., Fillion-Bilodeau, S., & Gosselin, F. (2008). The Montreal Affective Voices: a validated set of nonverbal affect bursts for research on auditory affective processing. Behavior Research Methods, 40(2), 531–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 843–859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., Lewis, V., & Collis, G. (1998). Familiar face and voice matching and recognition in children with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 39(2), 171–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., Lewis, V., & Collis, G. M. (2000). Voice processing abilities in children with autism, children with specific language impairments, and young typically developing children. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(7), 847–857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brennand, R., Schepman, A., & Rodway, P. (2011). Vocal emotion perception in pseudo-sentences by secondary-school children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1567–1573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charbonneau, G., Bertone, A., Lepore, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Mottron, L., & Collignon, O. (2013). Multilevel alterations in the processing of audio–visual emotion expressions in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 51(5), 1002–1010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chevallier, C., Noveck, I., Happé, F., & Wilson, D. (2011). What’s in a voice? Prosody as a test case for the Theory of Mind account of autism. Neuropsychologia, 49(3), 507–517.

  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65(6), 1663–1677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Boer-Schellekens, L., Keetels, M., Eussen, M., & Vroomen, J. (2013). No evidence for impaired multisensory integration of low-level audiovisual stimuli in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 51(14), 3004–3013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2007). Language, social behavior, and the quality of friendships in adolescents with and without a history of specific language impairment. Child Development, 78(5), 1441–1457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64(3), 657–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., et al. (2013). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 25(2), 298–312.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gebauer, L., Skewes, J., Hørlyck, L., & Vuust, P. (2014). Atypical perception of affective prosody in Autism Spectrum Disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 6, 370–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: a validation study. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1094–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. (2006). The Cambridge mindreading (CAM) face-voice battery: Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 169–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton, P., Reichenbacher, L., Sauter, D., Allen, R., Scott, S., & Hill, E. (2012). Measuring the effects of alexithymia on perception of emotional vocalizations in autistic spectrum disorder and typical development. Psychological Medicine, 42(11), 2453–2459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). Emotion recognition in autism: Coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18(4), 911–923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, K. H. S., & Iarocci, G. (2017). Are generalized anxiety and depression symptoms associated with social competence in children with and without autism spectrum disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(12), 3778–3788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, L., Hattersley, C., Molins, B., Buckley, C., Povey, C., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Which terms should be used to describe autism? Perspectives from the UK autism community. Autism, 20(4), 442–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: a possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(1), 29–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (1994). Learning and representation in speech and language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4(6), 812–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. (2005). Links between social and linguistic processing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmental Science, 8(1), F1–F12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Sourn-Bissaoui, S., Aguert, M., Girard, P., Chevreuil, C., & Laval, V. (2013). Emotional speech comprehension in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(4), 309–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 335–349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Pearson, D. A. (1995). Intermodal perception of affect in persons with autism or Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 7(3), 409–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Belgin Tunali-Kotoski, Y., Chen, R., Ortegon, J., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K. A., & Gibbs, M. C. (1997). Emotion recognition in autism: Verbal and nonverbal information. Development and Psychopathology, 9(3), 579–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, 91, 630.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., & Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: a critical review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 325–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., Peppé, S., Gibbon, F. E., O'Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Prosody and its relationship to language in school-aged children with high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42(6), 682–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Perception and production of prosody by speakers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 205–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1991). A neuroscientific perspec- tive on continuity and discontinuity in developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology: Models and integrations (pp. 117–159). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015–1028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M., Dahlstrom, B., & Squires, T. L. (1990). Autistic children's knowledge of thinking and feeling states in other people. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(4), 587–601.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., & Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Emotional prosody processing in autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(2), 224–239.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading the mind in the voice: A study with normal adults and adults with Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., et al. (2013). Motherese in interaction: at the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), e78103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shanker, S. (2004). Autism and the dynamic developmental model of emotions. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 11(3), 219–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, J. (2013). Why I dislike “person first” language. Autonomy, the Critical Journal of Interdisciplinary Autism Studies, 1(2).

  • Stephens, K., Nickerson, R., & Rollins, A. (1983). Suprasegmental and postural aspects of speech production and their effect on articulatory skills and intelligibility. In I. Hochberg, H. Levitt, & M. Osberger (Eds.), Speech of the hearing impaired: Research, training and personnel preparation (pp. 35–51). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Siemann, J. K., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Woynaroski, T. G., Camarata, S. M., & Wallace, M. T. (2014). Multisensory temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(3), 691–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ncube, B. L., Black, K. R., Bebko, J. M., Ferber, S., & Barense, M. D. (2018). The cascading influence of multisensory processing on speech perception in autism. Autism, 22(5), 609–624.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, M. E., McAdam, C., Ota, M., Peppé, S., & Cleland, J. (2013). Emotional recognition in autism spectrum conditions from voices and faces. Autism, 17(1), 6–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, J. W., & Sung, A. (2016). The “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(5), 1538–1552.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2005). Issues in the classification of autism and related conditions. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and developmental disorders (3rd ed., pp. 5–41). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc..

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. PsychCorp.

  • West, M. J., Copland, D. A., Arnott, W. L., Nelson, N. L., & Angwin, A. J. (2018). Effects of prosodic and semantic cues on facial emotion recognition in relation to autism-like traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(8), 2611–2618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yager, J., & Iarocci, G. (2013). The development of the multidimensional social competence scale: A standardized measure of social competence in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 6(6), 631–641.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge and thank the families who participated in this research, as well as the research assistants in the Autism and Developmental Disorders Lab who helped collect and analyze this data, in particular Hannah Visser and Troy Boucher. This work was supported by a grant from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) awarded to Grace Iarocci and a NSERC Post-doctoral award to Nichole Scheerer. Nichole Scheerer is also the recipient of a BrainsCAN Postdoctoral Fellowship at Western University, funded by the Canada First Research Excellence Fund (CFREF). Ryan Stevenson is funded by an NSERC Discovery Grant (RGPIN2017-04656), a SSHRC Insight Grant (435-2017-0936), the University of Western Ontario Faculty DevelopmentResearch Fund, and the John R. Evans Leaders Fund from the Canadian Foundation for Innovation (Project#37497)

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nichole E. Scheerer.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original online version of this article was revised: The original version of this article unfortunately contained a mistake. The image of Figure 1 should be corrected.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Scheerer, N.E., Shafai, F., Stevenson, R.A. et al. Affective Prosody Perception and the Relation to Social Competence in Autistic and Typically Developing Children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 48, 965–975 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00644-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00644-5

Keywords

Navigation