Skip to main content
Log in

Callous-Unemotional Behaviors and Harsh Parenting: Reciprocal Associations across Early Childhood and Moderation by Inherited Risk

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Callous-unemotional (CU) behaviors increase children’s risk for subsequent antisocial behavior. This risk process may begin in early childhood with reciprocal pathways between CU behaviors and harsh parenting. In a sample of 561 linked triads of biological mothers, adoptive parents, and adopted children, the present study examined bidirectional links between CU behaviors and harsh parenting across three time points from 18 to 54 months and investigated moderation by inherited risk for psychopathic traits. Child CU behaviors and harsh parenting were measured using adoptive mother and adoptive father reports, and biological mothers provided reports of their personality characteristics. Findings supported reciprocal associations between harsh parenting and CU behaviors during early childhood, especially during the transition from toddlerhood (27 months) to the preschool period (54 months). Moreover, multiple-group analyses showed that level of inherited risk moderated associations between CU behaviors and harsh parenting. Specifically, there were statistically reliable associations between CU behaviors at 27 months and adoptive mothers’ harsh parenting at 54 months, and between adoptive fathers’ harsh parenting at 27 months and CU behaviors at 54 months among children at higher inherited risk, but not among those at lower inherited risk. The findings illustrate the dynamic interplay between parenting, CU behaviors, and heritable risk.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because harsh parenting data at 54 months were missing for Cohort II, we reran the identical model but restricted analyses to Cohort I (n = 361). The path from CU behaviors at 27 months to harsh parenting at 54 months was statistically significant (β = .16, p = .01). The path from harsh parenting at 27 months to CU behaviors at 54 months was similar in magnitude but marginally significant (β = .11, p < .07), likely due to the reduced power associated with the smaller sample size.

  2. When we reran the identical model but restricted the sample to Cohort I (n = 361), the path from harsh parenting at 27 months to CU behaviors at 54 months continued to be statistically significant (β = .18, p < .01).

Abbreviations

CU:

Callous-unemotional

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, D. S., O'Leary, S. G., Wolff, L. S., & Acker, M. M. (1993). The parenting scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychological Assessment, 5, 137–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 81–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 5–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14, 786–799.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, J. D., Pardini, D. A., & Loeber, R. (2008). Reciprocal relationships between parenting behavior and disruptive psychopathology from childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 679–692.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Button, T. M. M., Lau, J. Y. F., Maughan, B., & Eley, T. C. (2008). Parental punitive discipline, negative life events and gene-environment interplay in the development of externalizing behavior. Psychological Medicine, 38, 29–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibtion, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Allen, J. L., McGregor, K., Woolgar, M., Viding, E., & Scott, S. (2014). Callous-unemotional traits in children and mechanisms of impaired eye contact during expressions of love: A treatment target? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 55, 771–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J. (2013). Moral development in early childhood and social interaction in the family. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 135–159). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enders, C. K. (2013). Dealing with missing data in developmental research. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 27–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flom, M., & Saudino, K. J. (2017). Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Genetic and environmmental contributions to stability and change. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 1227–1234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., & White, S. F. (2008). Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(4), 359–375. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610-2007-01862.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. (2014). Annual research review: A developmental psychopathology approach to understanding callous-unemotional traits in children and adolescents with serious conduct problems. Journal Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 532–548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Cadoret, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Yates, W., Troughton, E., & Stewart, M. A. (1996). The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 574–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for personality. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, L. W., Waller, R., Trentacosta, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., . . . Leve, L. D. (2016). Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 903–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kochanska, G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: From toddlerhood to age five. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laible, D., Thompson, R. A., & Froimson, J. (2014). Early socialization: The influence of close relationships. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (2nd ed., pp. 35–59). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Laird, R. D., Shaw, D. S., Petit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2011). Reciprocal relations between parents’ physical discipline and children’s externalizing behavior during middle childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 225–238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leve, L. D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Ganiban, J., Natsuaki, M. N., & Reiss, D. (2013). The early growth and development study: A prospective adoption study from birth through middle childhood. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 412–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorber, M. F., & Smith Slep, A. M. (2015). Are persistent early onset child conduct problems predicted by the trajectories and initial levels of discipline practices? Developmental Psychology, 51, 1048–1061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys' conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, 683–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1964). The psychopath: An essay on the criminal mind. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeil, T. F., Cantor-Graae, E., & Sjostrom, K. (1994). Obstetric complications as antecedents of schizophrenia: Empirical effects of using different obstetric complication scales. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 519–530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Messer, B., & Harter, S. (1986). Manual for the adult self-perception profile. Denver: University of Denver.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills-Koonce, W. R., Willoughby, M. T., Garrett-Peters, P., Wagner, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2016). The interplay among socioeconomic status, household chaos, and parenting in the prediction of child conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors. Development and Psychopathology, 28, 757–771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H., & Milne, B. J. (2002). Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: Follow-up at age 26 years. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 179–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthen, L., & Muthen, B. (2015). Mplus 7.31 (Statistical Program).

  • Newman, D. A. (2003). Longitudinal modeling with randomly and systematically missing data: A simulation of ad hoc, maximum likelihood, and multiple imputation techniques. Organizational Research Methods, 6, 328–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O'Leary, S. G., Slep, A. M. S., & Reid, M. J. (1999). A longitudinal study of mothers' overreactive discpline and toddlers' externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 331–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliver, B. R., Trzaskowski, M., & Plomin, R. (2014). Genetics of parenting: The power of the dark side. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1233–1240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pardini, D. A., Fite, P. J., & Burke, J. D. (2008). Bidirectional associations between parenting practices and conduct problems in boys from childhood to adolescence: The moderating effect of age and African-American ethnicity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 647–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach: Vol. 3 coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plamondon, A., Browne, D. T., Madigan, S., & Jenkins, J. M. (2018). Disentangling child-specific and family-wide processes underlying negative mother-child transactions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 437–447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sameroff, A. J. (2009). The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Scaramella, L. V., & Leve, L. D. (2004). Clarifying parent-child reciprocities during early childhood: The early childhood coercion model. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 7, 89–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype- environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D. S., & Bell, R. Q. (1993). Developmental theories of parental contributors to antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 493–518.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Cohn, J. F., & Bell, R. Q. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trentacosta, C. J., & Shaw, D. S. (2008). Maternal predictors of rejecting parenting and early adolescent antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 247–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viding, E., & McCrory, E. J. (2012). Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 969–983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., & Hyde, L. W. (2017). Callous–unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Measurement, meaning, and the influence of parenting. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 120–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., & Hyde, L. W. (2018). Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: The development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 11–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Gardner, F., Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2012). Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 946–953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Grabell, A. S., Alves, M. L., & Olson, S. L. (2015). Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: Multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 56, 657–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Trentacosta, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Reiss, D., . . . Hyde, L. H. (2016). Heritable temperament pathways to early callous-unemotional behavior. British Journal of Psychiatry, 209, 475–482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Shaw, D. S., & Hyde, L. W. (2017a). Observed fearlessness and positive parenting interact to predict childhood callous-unemotional behaviors among low-income boys. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 58, 282–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, R., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Reiss, D., . . . Hyde, L. W. (2017b). Toward an understanding of the role of the environment in the development of early callous behavior. Journal of Personality, 85, 90–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Waschbusch, D. A., Moore, G. A., & Propper, C. B. (2011). Using the ASEBA to screen for callous unemotional traits in early childhood: Factor structure, temporal stability, and utility. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 19–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, R., Propper, C. B., & Waschbusch, D. A. (2013). Observed parenting behaviors interact with a polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene to predict the emergence of oppositional defiant and callous-unemotional behaviors at age 3 years. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 903–917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Gottfredson, N. C., & Wagner, N. J. (2014). Measuring callous unemotional behaviors in early childhood: Factor structure and the prediction of stable aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 30–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The Early Growth and Development Study was supported by grants R01 HD042608 from NICHD, NIDA, and OBSSR, NIH, U.S. PHS (PI Years 1-5: David Reiss; PI Years 6-10: Leslie Leve), R01 DA020585 from NIDA, NIMH, and OBSSR, NIH, U.S. PHS (PI: Jenae Neiderhiser), R01 MH092118 from NIMH, NIH, U. S. PHS (PIs: Jenae Neiderhiser and Leslie Leve), and UG3 OD023389 from the Office of the Director, NIH, U.S. PHS (PIs: Leslie Leve, Jody Ganiban, and Jenae Neiderhiser). Christopher Trentacosta was supported by K01 MH082926 from NIMH, NIH, U.S. PHS. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The authors thank the biological parents and adoptive families who participated in this study and the adoption agencies who helped with the recruitment of study participants. They also gratefully acknowledge Rand Conger, John Reid, Xiaojia Ge, and Laura Scaramella for their contributions to the larger project.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher J. Trentacosta.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

The Early Growth and Development Study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of George Washington University, the Oregon Social Learning Center, the Pennsylvania State University, the University of California, Davis, the University of Minnesota, and the University of Oregon.

Informed Consent

Biological mothers and adoptive parents provided written informed consent. Adoptive parents also provided written informed consent for the adopted child.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Trentacosta, C.J., Waller, R., Neiderhiser, J.M. et al. Callous-Unemotional Behaviors and Harsh Parenting: Reciprocal Associations across Early Childhood and Moderation by Inherited Risk. J Abnorm Child Psychol 47, 811–823 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0482-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0482-y

Keywords

Navigation