Skip to main content
Log in

The Phenomenology of Non-Aggressive Antisocial Behavior During Childhood

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the phenomenology of overt or aggressive antisocial behavior during childhood is well-documented, far less is known about covert or non-aggressive, rule-breaking (RB) antisocial behavior. Gaps in knowledge include issues as basic as RB’s typical symptom presentation during childhood and which symptoms differ across sex. The current study sought to fill these gaps in the literature by establishing the prevalence and psychometric properties of specific RB behaviors in a sample of 1022 twin boys and 1010 twin girls between the ages of 6 and 10 years. Legal RB behaviors (e.g., breaking rules, swears, lying or cheating) were present to varying degrees in most children, regardless of whether or not they passed the clinical threshold for RB. They were also more common in boys than in girls regardless of their clinical status. In sharp contrast, illegal RB behaviors (e.g., stealing, vandalism, setting fires) were rarely observed in typically-developing children, but were seen at moderate levels in boys and girls with clinically-significant levels of RB. Moreover, sex differences in illegal RB behaviors were observed only for those youth with clinically meaningful levels of RB. Such findings collectively imply that while legal RB behaviors can be found (albeit at different frequencies) in children with and without clinically meaningful levels of RB, illegal RB behaviors may function as relatively ‘unambiguous’ indicator of clinically-significant levels of RB.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that these mean differences in conduct problems are largely specific to aggressive behaviors, and do not extend to non-aggressive rule-breaking (d = 0.067, ns). Thus, we collapsed the two samples for analysis herein to maximize statistical power and minimize reporting clutter.

  2. Although prior work has indicated that the frequencies of non-aggressive antisocial behaviors increase dramatically following puberty, we are not aware of consistent changes in frequency during the developmental period of middle childhood. Given this, along with the cross-sectional nature of our dataset, age was not a focus of the current analyses.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington,: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.

    Google Scholar 

  • Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, L. A., Barton, M., & Raine, A. (2002). The Southern California twin register at the University of Southern California. Twin Research, 5, 456–459.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, E. D., Seguin, J. R., White, H. R., Bates, M. E., Lacourse, E., Carbonneau, R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Developmental trajectories of male physical violence and theft: relations to neurocognitive performance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 592–599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 75, 1523–1537.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: a six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222–245.

  • Burt, S. A. (2009). Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 163–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A., & Klump, K. L. (2013). The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): an update. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 344–350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt, S. A., Rescorla, L. A., Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Almqvist, F., Begovac, I., & Verhulst, F. C. (2015). The association between aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial problems as measured with the Achenbach system of empirically based assessment: a study of 27,861 parent–adolescent dyads from 25 societies. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Côté, S. M., Vaillancourt, T., LeBlanc, J. C., Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). The development of physical aggression from toddlerhood to pre-adolescence: a nation wide longitudinal study of Canadian children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 71–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Côté, S. M., Vaillancourt, T., Barker, E. D., Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). The joint development of physical and indirect aggression: predictors of continuity and change during childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 37–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Embreton, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L., Van Horn, Y., Christ, M. A. G., et al. (1993). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: a meta-analytic review of factor analyses and cross-validation in a clinic sample. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(4), 319–340.

  • Hay, D. A., McStephen, M., Levy, F., & Pearsall-Jones, J. (2002). Recruitment and attrition in twin register studies of childhood behavior: the example of the Austrailian Twin ADHD Project. Twin Research, 5, 324–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, J. K., Silberg, J. L., Rutter, M., Simonoff, E., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H., et al. (1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 1. Phenotypic Assessment in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 943–963.

  • Klump, K. L., & Burt, S. A. (2006). The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): genetic, environmental, and neurobiological influences on behavior across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 971–977.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Externalizing psychopathology in adulthood: a dimensional-spectrum concepualization and its implications for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 537–550.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. J., & Lee, S. S. (2010). Latent class analysis of antisocial behavior: interaction of serotonin transporter genotype and maltreatment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 789–801.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: a research review and a research agenda. In B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), The causes of conduct disorder and serious juvenile delinquency (pp. 49–75). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monuteaux, M. C., Fitzmaurice, G., Blacker, D., Buka, S. L., & Biederman, J. (2004). Specificity in the familial aggregation of overt and covert conduct diosrder symptoms in a referred attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder sample. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1113–1127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998-2012). Mplus User's Guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Rescorla, L. A., Ginzburg, S., Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., Almqvist, F., Begovac, I., et al. (2013). Cross-informant agreement between parent-reported and adolescent self-reported problems in 25 societies. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42(2), 262–273. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.717870.

  • Robbers, S. C. C., Bartels, M., van Oort, F. V. A., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., et al. (2010). A twin-singleton comparison of developmental trajectories of externalizing and internalizing problems in 6- to 12-year-old children. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 13, 79–87.

  • Rutter, M., & Redshaw, J. (1991). Annotation: growing up as a twin: twin-singleton differences in psychological development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 885–895.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stranger, C., Achenbach, T. A., & Verhulst, F. C. (1997). Accelerated longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syndromes. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 43–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, R. E. (2003). Why socialization fails: the case of chronic physical aggression. In B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), The causes of conduct disorder and serious juvenile delinquency (pp. 182–224). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Developmental origins of disruptive behaviour problems: the 'original sin' hypothesis, epigenetics and their consequences for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 341–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Lier, P. A. C., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Koot, H. M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2009). Developmental links between trajectories of physical violence, vandalism, theft, and alcohol-drug use from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 481–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verhulst, F. C., & van der Ende, J. (1995). The eight-year stability of problem behavior in an epidemiologic sample. Pediatric Research, 38, 612–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wakschlag, L. S., Tolan, P. H., & Leventhal, B. L. (2010). Research review: 'Ain't misbehavin': towards a developmentally-specified nosology for preschool disruptive behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 3–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Walters, G. D. (2010). The latent structure of life-course persistent antisocial bheavior: is Moffitt's developmental taxonomy a true taxonomy? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0021519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, K. E., Ormel, J., & Krueger, R. F. (2011). The dimensional nature of externalizing behaviors in adolescence: evidence from a direct comparison of categorical, dimensional, and hybrid models. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 553–561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by R01-MH081813 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and by R01-HD066040 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, the NICHD, or the National Institutes of Health. The authors thank all participating twins and their families for making this work possible.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Alexandra Burt.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Burt, S.A., Brent Donnellan, M., Slawinski, B.L. et al. The Phenomenology of Non-Aggressive Antisocial Behavior During Childhood. J Abnorm Child Psychol 44, 651–661 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0076-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0076-x

Keywords

Navigation