Evolutionary Ecology

, Volume 21, Issue 5, pp 601–611 | Cite as

Colour perception in three-spined sticklebacks: sexes are not so different after all

Original Paper


Sexual selection theory implies a tight coupling between the evolution of male sexual display and the sensory capabilities of the female. In sexually dimorphic species it is proposed that this might lead to sex differences in a species’ perceptive abilities. However, supporting evidence for this is rare, and to date there is only one example; three-spined sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Here, the female’s visual system is reported to become more red-sensitive during the summer breeding season; a time when sexually mature males display a red throat and belly to potential mates. In contrast, a shift in sensitivity is not apparent in males. These results, although commonly quoted, are surprising because previous observations suggest that both sexes may benefit from the detection of the male’s red colour patch; in females the intensity of red coloration can influence the choice of mate, and in males the conspicuous red colouration can aid the detection of rival males. To investigate this paradox we repeated the original optomotor experiment using a fully controlled design. In contrast to the earlier result, we found that both males and females exhibit a shift in their sensitivity to red during the reproductive period. These new data therefore do not support the hypothesis that sex differences in perceptual abilities occur in sexually dimorphic species.


Vision Gasterosteus aculeatus Stickleback Receiver bias Sensory drive 



We thank Derek Cosens for his invaluable advice and practical assistance throughout the course of this study, Loch Lomond and The Trossachs National Park, Julian Partridge, and the Darwin Workshop for their help in the construction of experimental apparatus. We would also like to acknowledge the valuable input from two anonymous referees during the compilation of this paper. Funding for this experiment came from Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council.


  1. Andersson M (1994) Sexual selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, pp 3–52Google Scholar
  2. Bakker TCM, Mundwiler B (1994) Female mate choice and male red coloration in a natural three-spine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) population. Behav Ecol 5:74–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beatty DD (1966) A study of the succession of visual pigments in Pacific salmon (Oncorhynchus). Can J Zool 44:429–455PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Boughman JW (2001) Divergent sexual selection enhances reproductive isolation in sticklebacks. Nature 411:944–947PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boulcott, Pand Braithwaite VA (2005) Ultraviolet light and visual behaviour in the three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 78:736–743PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Braithwaite VA, Barber I (2000) Limitations to colour based sexual preferences in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 47:413–416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bridges CDB (1972) The rhodopsin-porphyropsin visual system. In: Dartnall HJA (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, Vol. VII/1. Springer Verlag, New York, pp 471–480Google Scholar
  8. Capranica RR (1976) Morphology and physiology of the auditory system. In: Llinas R, Precht W (eds) Frog neurobiology. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp 551–575Google Scholar
  9. Cronin TW, Jarvilehto M, Weckstrom M, Lall AB (2000) Tuning of photoreceptor sensitivity in fireflies (Coleoptera: Lampyridae). J Comp Physiol A 186:1–12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cronly-Dillon JR, Sharma SC (1968) Effect of season and sex on the photopic sensitivity of the three-spined stickleback. J Exp Biol 49:679–687PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Douglas RH, Hawryshyn CW (1990) Behavioural studies of fish vision: an analysis of visual capabilities. In: Douglas RH, Djamgoz MBA (eds) The Visual System of Fish. Chapman and Hall, London, pp 279–343Google Scholar
  12. Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat. 139:S125–S153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Endler JA (1993) Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos T Roy Soc B 340:215–225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Endler JA, McLellan T (1988) The processes of evolution: towards a newer synthesis. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 19:395–421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fisher RA (1915) The evolution of sexual preference. Eugen Rev 7:184–192Google Scholar
  16. Fleishman LJ (1988) Sensory influences on physical design of a visual display. Anim Behav 36:1420–1424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hoy RR (1978) Acoustic communication in crickets: A model system for the study of feature detection. Fed Proc 37:2316–2323PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Kirkpatrick M (1982) Sexual selection and the evolution of female choice. Evolution 36:1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kröger RHH, Knoblauch B, Wagner H-J (2003) Rearing in different photic and spectral environments changes the optomotor response to chromatic stimuli in cichlid fish (Aequidens pulcher). J Exp Biol 206:1643–1648PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Knowles A, Dartnall HJA (1977) The photobiology of vision. In Davson H (ed) The eye. Academic Press, London , pp 247–497Google Scholar
  21. Lorenz K (1970) Studies in animal and human behaviour. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 403 ppGoogle Scholar
  22. McDonald CG, Hawryshyn CW (1995) Intraspecific variation of the spectral sensitivity in the three-spine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) from different photic regimes. J Comp Physiol A 176:255–260CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. McLennan DA, McPhail JD (1990) Experimental investigations of the evolutionary significance of sexually dimorphic nuptial coloration in Gasterosteus aculeatus (L.): the relationship between male colour and female behaviour. Can J Zool 68:482–492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Muntz WRA, Northmore DPM (1973) Scotopic spectral sensitivity in a teleost fish, (Scardinius erythrophthalmus) adapted to different daylengths. Vision Res 13:245–252PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Muntz WBA, Wainwright AW (1978) Annual cycles in the light environments and visual mechanisms of fishes. In: Thorpe JE (ed) Rhythmic activity of fishes. Academic Press: London, pp 105–129Google Scholar
  26. Ostlund-Nilsson S, Holmlund M (2003) The artistic three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteous aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:214–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Partridge JC, Cummings ME (1999) Adaptation of visual pigments to the aquatic environment. In: Archer SN, Djamgoz MBA, Loew ER, Partridge JC, Valerga S (eds) Adaptive mechanisms in the ecology of vision. Kluwer Academic Publishers, London, pp 251–284Google Scholar
  28. Pelkwijk JJ Ter, Tinbergen N (1937) Eine reizbiologische Analyse einiger Verhaltensweisen von Gasterosteus aculeatus L. Z Tierz Züchtungsbio 1:193–204Google Scholar
  29. Reimchen TE (1989) Loss of nuptial colour in three-spine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Evolution 43:450–460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Reimchen TE (1994) Predators and morphological evolution in threespine stickleback. In: Bell MA, Foster SA (eds) The evolutionary ecology of the threespine stickleback. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 240–273Google Scholar
  31. Rick IP, Modarressie R, Bakker TCM (2006) UV wavelengths affect female mate choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 71:307–313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Rowe MP, Baube CL, Loew ER, Phillips JB (2004) Optimal mechanisms for finding and selecting mates: how threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) should encode male throat colors. J Comp Physiol A 190:241–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rowland WJ (1994) Proximate determinants of stickleback behaviour: an evolutionary perspective. In: Bell MA, Foster SA (eds) The evolutionary ecology of the threespine stickleback. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 297–344Google Scholar
  34. Ryan MJ (1990a) Signals, species and sexual selection. Am Sci 78:46–52Google Scholar
  35. Ryan MJ (1990b) Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation. In: Futuyma D, Antonovics J (eds) Oxford surveys in evolutionary biology, vol 7. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 157–195Google Scholar
  36. Sargent RC, Rush VN, Wisenden BD, Yan HY (1998) Courtship and mate choice in fishes: integrating behavioural and sensory ecology. Am Zool 38:82–96Google Scholar
  37. Scott RJ (2001) Sensory drive and nuptial colour loss in the three-spined stickleback. J Fish Biol 59:1520–1528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Smith C, Barber I, Wootton RJ, Chittka L (2004) A receiver bias in the origin of three-spined stickleback mate choice. Proc Roy Soc B 271:949–955CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Thorpe A, Douglas RH (1993) Spectral transmission and short-wave absorbing pigments in the fish lens—II. Effects of age. Vision Res 33:301–307PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tinbergen N (1951) (1989) The study of instinct. Clarendon, Oxford, 154 ppGoogle Scholar
  41. Wootton RJ, Smith C (2000) A long-term study of a short-lived fish: the demography of Gasterosteus aculeatus. Behaviour 137:981–997CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute for Evolutionary Biology, School of Biological SciencesThe University of EdinburghEdinburghUK

Personalised recommendations