Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

What drives the perception of health and safety risks in the workplace? Evidence from European labour markets

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Empirica Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Worker perceptions of job-related health risk are a little-studied dimension of heterogeneity in the labour market. According to information from the European Working Conditions Survey (EWCS), one out of three European workers considers that her health and safety is at risk because of work. Not surprisingly, risk perceptions are influenced by objective risk factors such as hazardous working conditions, onerous job characteristics and by the probability to be affected by occupational accidents and illnesses. This paper explores also the role played by personal characteristics and household structure for the explanation of risk perceptions. After controlling for job characteristics, workplace hazards, job satisfaction and health outcomes, I find that risk perceptions are strongly correlated with gender, age, and household structure. Lone parents as well as older and more experienced workers have a higher propensity than other categories to consider their health at risk because of work. The same seems to hold true for better educated workers, especially for those who have completed tertiary education. Further results suggest that the relationship between household structure and risk perception is stable across gender.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a (partial) review of this literature see for instance Sarin and Weber (1993), Sjöberg (2000) and Bouyer et al. (2001).

  2. Another instance is represented by the fact that the majority of drivers tend not to equate their own traffic risk with that of the average person, because they believe themselves to be more skilful and safer than average (see Matthews and Moran 1986).

  3. For a synthetic overview see Bouyer et al. (2001).

  4. The exact wording of the question is: “Do you think that your health and safety is at risk because of your work?”.

  5. I have constructed variables for different household typologies, accounting for the presence of children below the age of 18 and differentiating between singles, couples without children, families and lone parents.

  6. Each dummy variable takes the value of 1 if the respondent stated that he or she is exposed to the condition “around half of the time” or more, and of 0 otherwise.

  7. Although the table displays three aggregated age cohorts, the age variables included in the model are expressed in years. In order to construct the household variables, dependents up to the age of 18 who live with one or both of their parents were counted as children. The educational dummies are based on the international ISCED classification.

  8. A comparison with the third wave of the EWCS, where only the EU15 were surveyed, shows that this share has remained virtually unchanged between 2000 and 2005 (see European Foundation 2005).

  9. This calculation includes Romania and Bulgaria among the new EU member states, although technically they were not part of the Union at the moment when the survey was carried out.

  10. According to the statistical agency of the European Union, in 2005 the incidence rate of deadly work accidents for every 100,000 workers was 2.3 in the EU15 and 2.6 in the EU27.

  11. I present the results in this form because it corresponds to the regression output provided by STATA to zero-inflated count data models.

  12. On the other hand, it cannot be entirely ruled out that the observed variables fail to account fully for gender differences in occupational sorting, i.e. for the fact that women might sort into safer jobs even when they work in the same industry and occupation as men and are exposed to similar working conditions.

  13. Since the difference between actual and predicted risk perception lies in the range between −1 and 1, I implement the following transformation: When the difference (i.e. predicted value minus actual value) is larger than 0.5 I assume that there is a low risk perception on the part of the individual; when the difference is below −0.5 I assume a high risk perception; values between −0.5 and 0.5 represent the baseline of the subsequent analysis, as I assume that the individual response coincides with what is predicted by the model.

  14. In this specification I do not include any other control variables. Inclusion of control variables (country, industry, occupation and firm size) and of the variables for personal characteristics and satisfaction leads to results which are almost identical to those displayed in Table 4. I take this as evidence that results are robust to different specifications and transformations of the information on working conditions and job characteristics.

References

  • Akerlof GA, Dickens WT (1982) The economic consequences of cognitive dissonance. Am Econ Rev 72(3):307–319

    Google Scholar 

  • Barone A, Nese A (2003) On the job health risks: workers’ beliefs and individual work experiences. Giornale degli Economisti e Annali di Economia 62(1):125–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender KA, Mridha HA, Peoples J (2006) Risk compensation for hospital workers: evidence from relative wages of janitors. Ind Labor Relat Rev 59(2):226–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Böckerman P, Ilmakunnas P (2006) Do job disamenities raise wages or ruin job satisfaction? Int J Manpow 27(3):290–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouyer M, Bagdassarian S, Chaabanne S, Mullet E (2001) Personality correlates of risk perception. Risk Anal 21(3):457–465

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case A, Paxson C (2005) Sex differences in morbidity and mortality. Demography 42(2):189–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson DJ, Freudenburg WR (1996) Gender and environmental concerns: a review and analysis of available research. Environ Behav 28:302–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLeire T, Levy H (2004) Worker sorting and the risk of death on the job. J Labor Econ 22(4):925–953

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado MA, Kiesner TJ (1997) Count data models with variance of unknown form: an application to a hedonic model of worker absenteeism. Rev Econ Stat 79(1):41–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dionne G, Dostie B (2008) New evidence on the determinants of absenteeism using linked employer-employee data. Ind Labor Relat Rev 61(1):108–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Dionne G, Fluet C, Desjardins D (2007) Predicted risk perception and risk-taking behaviour: the case of impaired driving. J Risk Uncertain 35:237–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott RF, Sandy R (1998) Adam Smith may have been right after all: a new approach to the analysis of compensating differentials. Econ Lett 59:127–131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions (2005) Third European Working Conditions Survey 2000, Dublin

  • European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions (2007a) Quality report of the 4th European Working Conditions Survey, Dublin

  • European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions (2007b) Fourth European Working Conditions Survey 2005, Dublin

  • Fischhoff B, Slovic P, Lichtenstein S, Read S, Combs B (1978) How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sci 8:127–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frick B, Malo MA (2008) Labour market institutions and individual absenteeism in the European Union. Ind Relat 47(4):505–529

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaba W, Viscusi WK (1998) Differences in subjective risk thresholds: worker groups as an example. Manage Sci 44(6):801–811

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garen J (1988) Compensating wage differentials and the endogeneity of job riskiness. Rev Econ Stat 70(1):9–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grazier S, Sloane PJ (2008) Accident risk, gender, family status and occupational choice in the UK. Labour Econ 15(5):938–957

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson E (1998) Gender differences in risk perception: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Risk Anal 18(6):805–811

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hakes JK, Viscusi WK (2004) Dead reckoning: demographic determinants of the accuracy of mortality risk perceptions. Risk Anal 24(3):651–664

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsee CK, Weber EU (1998) Cross-cultural differences in risk perception, but cross-cultural similarities in attitudes towards perceived risk. Manage Sci 44(9):1205–1214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson E, Tversky A (1984) Representations of perceptions of risks. J Exp Psychol 113(1):55–70

    Google Scholar 

  • Lalive R (2003) Did we overestimate the value of health? J Risk Uncertain 27(2):171–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leeth JD, Ruser J (2003) Compensating wage differentials for fatal and nonfatal injury risk by gender and race. J Risk Uncertain 27(3):257–277

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtenstein S, Slovic P, Fishhoff B, Layman M, Combs B (1978) Judged frequency of lethal events. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 4(6):551–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundborg P, Lindgren B (2004) Do they know what they are doing? Risk perceptions and smoking behaviour among Swedish teenagers. J Risk Uncertain 28(3):261–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews ML, Moran AR (1986) Age differences in male drivers perception of accident risk: the role of perceived driving abilities. Accid Anal Prev 18(4):299–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson V, Stewart T (2004) Risk perception in relation to musculoskeletal disorders. Health & Safety Executive, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarin RK, Weber M (1993) Risk-value models. Eur J Oper Res 70(2):135–149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage I (1993) An empirical investigation into the effect of psychological perceptions on the willingness-to-pay to reduce risk. J Risk Uncertain 6(1):75–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sjöberg L (2000) Factors in risk perception. Risk Anal 20(1):1–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sjöberg L, B-E Moen, T Rundmo (2004) Explaining risk perception. An evaluation of the psychometric paradigm in risk perception research, Rotunde, 84, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim

  • Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S (1982) Why study risk perceptions? Risk Anal 2(2):83–93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic P, Fischhoff B, Lichtenstein S (1985) Characterizing perceived risk. In: Kates RW, Hohenemser C, Kasperson J (eds) Perilous progress: managing the hazards of technology. Westview, Boulder, pp 91–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi WK (1989) Prospective reference theory: toward an explanation of the paradoxes. J Risk Uncertain 2:235–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi WK (1990) Sources of inconsistency in societal responses to health risks. Am Econ Rev 80(2):257–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Viscusi WK, Hakes JK (2008) Risk beliefs and smoking behavior. Econ Inq 46(1):45–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber EU, Millman R (1997) Perceived risk attitudes: relating risk perception to risky choice. Manage Sci 43:122–143

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelmann R (1999) Wages, firm size and absenteeism. Appl Econ Lett 6(6):337–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank René Böheim, Martin Falk and Andrea Weber as well as two anonymous referees for very useful comments to earlier drafts of this article. Responsibility for the final product lies entirely with me. I am also grateful to participants of the NoeG 2009 conference in Linz for their comments and to the European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions for providing the EWCS data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Leoni.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 5.

Table 5 Descriptive statistics

Appendix 2

See Table 6.

Table 6 Estimation with sample disaggregated by gender

Appendix 3

3.1 Principal component analysis

See Tables 7 and 8; Fig. 2.

Table 7 Probit model with factors from PCA as independent variables
Table 8 Multinomial logit to identify relationship between personal characteristics and systematic deviations between actual and predicted risk perceptions
Fig. 2
figure 2

Scree plot after PCA for working conditions and job characteristics

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Leoni, T. What drives the perception of health and safety risks in the workplace? Evidence from European labour markets. Empirica 37, 165–195 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10663-010-9129-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10663-010-9129-0

Keywords

JEL classification

Navigation