Advertisement

Sex Differences in Physical and Indirect Aggression: A Developmental Perspective

  • Sylvana M. Côté
Article

Abstract

Males generally use aggression more often than females. However, the magnitude of difference between the sexes varies widely according to the type of aggression that is considered, and according to the developmental period studied. Taking a developmental perspective, this paper reviews research that compares the progression of physical aggression (predominantly used by males) with indirect aggression (predominantly used by females) among males and females. Existing empirical evidence indicates that most children cease to use physical aggression during the course of childhood, but that a minority fails to do so. This group is comprised of children with high, stable levels of PA and is mostly male. Overall, most children use low levels of IA, but there is one group that uses this type of aggression with increasing frequency. This group is mostly female. Importantly, the differences between the sexes are not stable over time. Rather, while the gap between males and females is present during preschool years, it widens considerably during childhood and preadolescence. A review of hypotheses based on evolution, biology and social learning provides critical insight into the origins and development of sex differences in aggression over the life course. We conclude by arguing that violence in males may be effectively reduced through early, sustained intervention with high-risk mothers.

Keywords

Sex differences Physical aggression Indirect aggression Development Antisocial behaviors 

References

  1. Alink, L. R. A., van Zeijl, J., Stolk, M. N., Juffer, F., Koot, H. M., Bakersman-Kranenburg, M. J., et al. (2006). The Early Childhood Aggression Curve: Development of Physical Aggression in 10- to 50-Months-Old Children. Child Development, 77(4), 954–966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR).Google Scholar
  3. Archer, J. (1994). Violence between men. In J. Archer (Ed.), Male violence (pp. 121–140). London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  4. Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 651–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Archer, J., & Parker, S. (1994). Social representations of aggression in children. Aggressive Behavior, 20(2), 101–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Archer, J., & Côté, S. (2005). Sex differences in aggressive behavior: A developmental and evolutionary perspective. In R. E. Tremblay, W. H. Hartup & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  7. Archer, J., & Coyne, J. C. (2005). Why are girls more socially aggressive than boys? An integrated review of indirect, relational, and social aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(3), 212–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baillargeon, R. H., Zoccolillo, M., Keenan, K., Côté, S. M., Pérusse, D., Boivin, M., et al. (2007). Gender differences in the prevalence of physical aggression: A prospective population-based survey of children before and after two years of age. Developmental Psychology, 43(1), 13–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  10. Bjorklund, D. J., & Pelligrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. Washington D.C.: APA.Google Scholar
  11. Björkqvist, K. (1994). Sex differences in physical, verbal and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles, 30, 177–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bjorkqvist, K. (2001). Different names, same issue. Social Development, 10, 272–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K. M. J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992a). Do girls manipulate and boys fight?: Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Björkqvist, K., Oesterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992b). The development of direct and indirect aggressive strategies in males and females. In K. Björkqvist & P. Niemelae (Eds.), Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression (pp. 51–64). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.Google Scholar
  15. Borge, A., Rutter, M., Côté, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2004). Early childcare and physical aggression: differentiating social selection and social causation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 367–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Brame, B., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Developmental trajectories of physical aggression from school entry to late adolescence. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 389–394.Google Scholar
  17. Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K., et al. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six site, cross national study. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 222–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Ferguson, L. L., & Gariépy, J. L. (1989). Growth and aggression: 1. Childhood to early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 320–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cahill, L. (2005). His brain, her brain. [Electronic Version]. Scientific American.com.Google Scholar
  20. Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women’s intrasexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(2), 203–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E. P., Sparling, J., & Miller-Johnson, S. (2002). Early childhood education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecederian project. Applied Developmental Science, 6, 42–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Côté, S., Vaillancourt, T., LeBlanc, J. C., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). The development of physical aggression from toddlerhood to pre-adolescence: A nation wide longitudinal study of Canadian children. Journal of Abnomal Child Psychology, 34(11), 71–85.Google Scholar
  24. Côté, S., Vaillancourt, T., Barker, E. D., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Predictors of continuity and change in the joint development of physical and indirect aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 19(1), 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Côté, S. M., Boivin, M., Nagin, D. S., Japel, C., Xu, Q., Zoccolillo, M., et al. (in press a). The role of maternal education and non-maternal care services in the prevention of children’s physical aggression. Archives of General Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  26. Côté, S., Borge, A., Rutter, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (in press b). Nonmaternal care in infancy and emotional/behavioral difficulties at school entry: Moderation by family and infant. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
  27. Courtwright, D. T. (1996). Violent land: Single men and social disorder from the frontier to the inner city. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Crick, N. R. (1995). Relational aggression: The role of intent attributions, feelings of distress, and provocation types. Development and Psychopathology, 7(2), 313–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66(3), 710–722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Crick, N. R., Ostrov, J. M., Appleyard, K., Jansen, E. A., & Casas, J. F. (2004). Relational aggression in early childhood: You can’t come to my birthday party unless. In M. Putallaz & K. L. Bierman (Eds.), Aggression, antisocial behavior, and violence among girls: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  31. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1990). Killing the competition: Female/female male/male homicide. Human Nature, 1, 81–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Fagot, B. I., & Hagan, R. (1985). Aggression in toddlers: Responses to the assertive acts of boys and girls. Sex Roles, 12, 341–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Feshbach, S. (1970). Aggression. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology (Vol. 2, 3rd edition). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  34. Francis, D., Diorio, J., Liu, D., & Meaney, M. J. (1999). Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. Science, 286(5442), 1155–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (2002). Environmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 7840–7843.Google Scholar
  36. Francis, D. D., Szegda, K., Campbell, G., Martin, W. D., & Insel, T. (2003). Epigenetic sources of behavioral differences in mice. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 445–446.Google Scholar
  37. Galen, B. R., & Underwood, M. K. (1997). A developmental investigation of social aggression among children. Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 589–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Geoffroy, M.-C., Côté, S. M., Parent, S., & Séguin, J. R. (2006). Day-care attendance, stress and mental health. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 51(9), 607–615.Google Scholar
  39. Geoffroy, M.-C., Côté, S., Borge, A., Larouche, F., Séguin, J. R., & Rutter, M. (2007). Association between nonmaternal care in the first year of life and children’s receptive language skills prior to school entry: The moderating role of the socioeconomic status. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
  40. Goldstein, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Horton, N. J., Makris, N., Kennedy, D. N., Caviness Jr., V. S., et al. (2001). Normal Sexual Dimorphism of the Adult Human Brain Assessed by In Vivo Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Cerebral Cortoex, 11(6), 490–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Green, L. R., Richardson, D. R., & Lago, T. (1996). How do friendship, indirect, and direct aggression relate? Aggressive Behavior, 22, 81–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Haberstick, B. C., Smolen, A., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Family-based association test of the 5HTTLPR and aggressive behavior in a general population sample of children. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 836–843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Halperin, J. M., Kalmar, J. H., Schulz, K. P., Marks, D. J., Sharma, V., & Newcorn, J. H. (2006). Elevated Childhood Serotonergic Function Protects Against Adolescent Aggression in Disruptive Boys. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(7) 833–840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Harper, T. (2005). Fetal growth restriction [Electronic Version]. e-Medicine.Google Scholar
  45. Hay, D. F., Castle, J., & Davies, L. (2000). Toddlers’ use of force against familiar peers: A precursor of serious aggression? Child Development, 71, 457–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Higley, J. D., Mehlman, P. T., Taub, D. M., Higley, S. B., Suomi, S. J., & Linnoila, M. (1996). Stability of interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to severe aggression and competent social behavior in rhesus Macaque females. Neuropsychopharmacology, 7, 67–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Hrdy, S. (1999). Mother nature: A history of mother’s infants, and natural selection. New York: Patheon BooksGoogle Scholar
  48. Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E. M., Kasen, S., & Brook, J. S. (2002). Television viewing and aggressive behavior during adolescence and adulthood. Science, 295, 2468–2471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Keenan, K., & Shaw, D. (1997). Developmental and social influences on young girls’ early problem behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 95–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Keenan, K., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2000). More than the terrible twos: The nature and severity of behavior problems in clinic-referred preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(1), 33–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Kippin, T. E., Cain, S. W., Masum, Z., & Ralph, M. R. (2004). Neural stem cells show bidirectional experience-dependent plasticity in the perinatal mammalian brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2832–2836.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Lagerspetz, K. M., Bjorkqvist, K., & Peltonen, T. (1988). Is indirect aggression typical of females: Gender differences in aggressiveness in 11- to 12-years-old children. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 403–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1998). Development of juvenile aggression and violence. Some common misconceptions and controversies. American Psychologist, 53, 242–259.Google Scholar
  55. Maccoby, E. (1998). The two sexes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  56. McGrew, W. C. (1972). An ethological study of children’s behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  57. Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual review of neuroscience, 24, 1161–1192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. (2001). Sex differences in antisocial behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Nagin, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically violent and non violent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70(5), 1181–1196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. National Institute for Child Health Development [NICHD], & Early Childhood Care Research [E. C. C. R.] Network (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle school. Monographs of the Society for Reasearch in Child Development, Serial no.278 69-4 Google Scholar
  61. NICHD (2005a). Characteristics of infant child care: Factors contributing to positive caregiving. In NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (Ed.), Child Care and Child Development. Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (pp. 50–66). New York, London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  62. NICHD (2005b). Early child care and children’s development prior to school entry: results from the NICHD study of early child care. In NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (Ed.), Child Care and Child Development. Results from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. (pp. 376–392). New York, London: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  63. Nishizawa, S., Benkelfat, C., Young, S. N., Leyton, M., Mzengeza, S., de Montigny, C., et al. (1997). Differences between males and females in rates of serotonin synthesis in human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (Washington, DC), 94, 5308–5313 (May).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Olds, D., Henderson, C. R., Cole, R., Eckenrode, J., Kitzman, H., Luckey, D., et al. (1998). Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior: Fifteen-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 1238–1244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Olds, D. L., Henderson, C. R., Jr., Kitzman, H., Eckenrode, J., Cole, R., & Tatelbaum, R. C. (1999). Prenatal and infancy home visitation by nurses: Recent findings. The Future of Children, 9, 44–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Olds, D. L., Robinson, J., O’Brien, R., Luckey, D. W., Pettitt, L. M., Henderson, C. R., et al. (2002). Home visiting by nurses and by paraprofessionals: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 110(3), 486–496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Ostrov, J. M., & Keating, C. F. (2004). Gender differences in preschool aggression during free play and structured interactions: An observational study. Social Development, 13, 255–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Bank, L. (1984). Family interaction: A process model of deviancy training. Aggressive Behavior, 10, 253–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Peisner-Feinberg, E. S., Burchinal, M., Clifford, R. M., Culkin, M., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L., et al. (2001). The relation of preschool child-care quality to children’s cognitive and social development trajectories through second grade. Child Development, 72, 1534–1553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Quetelet, A. (1984). Research on the propensity for crime at different ages (S. F. Sylvester, Trans.). Cincinnnati, OH: Anderson (Original work published in 1833).Google Scholar
  71. Reiss, A. J., & Roth, J. A. (Eds.). (1993). Understanding and preventing violence. Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
  72. Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, S. E. (2002). Risky family: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Schweinhart, L. L., Barnes, H. V., & Weikart, D. P. (1993). Significant benefits. The High/Scope Perry School Study through age 27. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.Google Scholar
  74. Sears, R. R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1965). Identification and childrearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  75. Straus, M. A., & Gelles, R. J. (1990). Physical violence in American families: Risk factors and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  76. Suomi, S. J., & Levine, S. (1998). Psychobiology of intergenerational effects of trauma: Evidence from animal studies. In Y. Danieli (Ed.), Intergenerational handbook of multigeneration legacies (pp. 623–637). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  77. Suomi, S. J. (2003). Social and biological mechanisms underlying impulsive aggressiveness in rhesus monkeys. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York: Guilford press.Google Scholar
  78. Suomi, S. J. (2005). Genetic and environmental factors influencing the expression of impulsive aggression and serotonergic functioning in rhesus monkeys. In R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 63–82). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  79. Thornberry, T. (1998). Membership in youth gangs and involvement in serious and violent offending. In R. Loeber & D. P. Farrington (Eds.), Serious and violent juvenile offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions (pp. 147–166). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  80. Tremblay, R. E., Boulerice, B., Harden, P. W., McDuff, P., Pérusse, D., Pihl, R. O., et al. (1996). Do children in Canada become more aggressive as they approach adolescence? In Human Resources Development Canada & Statistics Canada (Eds.), Growing up in Canada: National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (pp. 127–137). Ottawa: Statistics Canada.Google Scholar
  81. Tremblay, R. E., Japel, C., Pérusse, D., McDuff, P., Boivin, M., Zoccolillo, M., et al. (1999). The search for the age of «onset» of physical aggression: Rousseau and Bandura revisited. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 9, 8–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Tremblay, R. E. (2000). The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in the past century? International Journal of Behavioral development, 24(2), 129–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Tremblay, R. E. (2003). Why socialization fails? The case of chronic physical aggression. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 182–224). New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
  84. Tremblay, R. E., Nagin, D. S., Séguin, J. R., Zoccolillo, M., Zelazo, P., Boivin, M., et al. (2004). Physical aggression during early childhood: Trajectories and predictors. Pediatrics, 114(1), e43–e50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Underwood, M. K. (2003). Social aggression among girls (p. 300). New York, NY, US: Guiford Press.Google Scholar
  86. Underwood, M. K., Galen, B. R., & Paquette, J. A. (2001). Top ten challenges for understanding gender and aggression in children: Why can’t we all just get along? Social Development, 10, 248–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Vaillancourt, T. (2005). Indirect aggression among humans: Social construct or evolutionary adaptation? In R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup & J. Archer (Eds.), Developmental origins of aggression (pp. 158–177). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  88. Vaillancourt, T., Miller, J., Fagbemi, J., Côté, S. M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2007). Trajectories and Predictors of Indirect Aggression: Results from a nationally representative longitudinal study of Canadian children aged 2 to 8. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Virkkunen, M., Goldman, D., Nielsen, D. A., & Linnoila, M. (1995). Low brain serotonin turnover rate (low CSF 5-HIAA) and impulsive violence. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 20(4), 271–275.Google Scholar
  90. Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847–854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Westergaard, C. G., Suomi, S. J., Chavanne, T. J., Houser, L., Hurlye, A. C., Cleveland, A., et al. (2003). Physiological correlates of aggression and impulsivity in free-ranging female primates. Neuropsychopharmacology, 28, 1045–1055.Google Scholar
  92. Zoccolillo, M., Paquette, D., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Maternal conduct disorder and the risk for the next generation. In D. Pepler, K. Masden, C. Webster & K. Levene (Eds.), Development and treatment of girlhood aggression (pp. 225–252). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science + Business Media B.V. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.GRIPUniversité de MontréalMontréal, QuébecCanada
  2. 2.Social and Preventive MedicineUniversity of MontréalMontréal, QuébecCanada
  3. 3.Faculty of Social SciencesUtrecht UniversityUtrechtNetherlands

Personalised recommendations