Cognitive Therapy and Research

, Volume 35, Issue 6, pp 536–543 | Cite as

Emotion Regulation in Depression: Reflection Predicts Recovery from a Major Depressive Episode

Brief Report


Little is known about the relation between individual differences in emotion regulation (ER) and the maintenance of clinical depression. This study examined whether frequency of use of four ER strategies (i.e., cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression, reflection, and brooding) predicts recovery from a major depressive episode. At an initial appointment (Time 1), participants diagnosed with current major depressive disorder completed measures assessing symptom severity and use of ER strategies. Six months later (Time 2), participants were reassessed to determine diagnostic status (i.e., recovered or non-recovered). Results demonstrated that, after controlling for symptom severity, use of ER strategies predicted recovery status at Time 2. Specifically, use of reflection at Time 1 was a unique and significant predictor of greater chance for recovery. Results indicate that ER strategies may be utilized to predict long-term symptom maintenance and provide support for the proposition that reflection may be used adaptively among individuals diagnosed with depression.


Depression Emotion regulation Rumination Reflection 


  1. Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  3. Bagby, R. M., Rector, N. A., Bacchiochi, J. R., & McBride, C. (2004). The stability of the response styles questionnaire rumination scale in a sample of patients with major depression. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 28, 527–538. doi: 0147-5916/04/0800-0527/0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, T. A., Steer, R. A., Ball, R., & Ranieri, W. F. (1996a). Comparison of the beck depression inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 588–597.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beck, T. A., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996b). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  6. Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: The ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15, 482–487. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00705.x.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Campbell-Sills, L., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Dalgleish, T., Yiend, J., Schweizer, S., & Dunn, B. D. (2009). Ironic effects of emotion suppression when recounting distressing memories. Emotion, 9, 744–749. doi: 10.1037/a0017290.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Donaldson, C., & Lam, D. (2004). Rumination, mood and social problem-solving in major depression. Psychological Medicine, 34, 1309–1318. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704001904.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion, 10, 563–572. doi: 10.1037/a0019010.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders, research version, patient edition (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
  12. Gross, J. J., & John, O. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269–280. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.002.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 281–298. doi: 10.1080/02699930903407948.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Mood regulation in depression: Differential effects of distraction and recall of happy memories on sad mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 484–490. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.484.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Keller, M., Lavori, P., Mueller, T., & Endicott, J. (1992). Time to recovery, chronicity, and levels of psychopathology in major depression: A 5-year prospective follow-up of 431 subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 809–816.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Lo, C. S. L., Ho, S. M. Y., & Hollon, S. D. (2008). The effects of rumination and negative cognitive styles on depression: A mediation analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.01.013.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 504511. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Pearson, K. A., Watkins, E. R., Mullan, E. G., & Moberly, N. J. (2010). Psychosocial correlates of depressive rumination. Behaviour Research and Therapy. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.007.
  21. Ray, R. D., McRae, K., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Cognitive reappraisal of negative affect: Converging evidence from EMG and self-report. Emotion, 10, 587–592. doi: 10.1037/a0019015.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Skre, I., Onstad, S., Torgensen, S., & Kringlen, E. (1991). High interrater reliability for the structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R axis I (SCID-I). Acta Psyhiatrica Scandinavica, 84, 167–173. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb03123.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MiamiCoral GablesUSA

Personalised recommendations