1 Introduction

Although studies have explored the effects of weekends on securities prices (Chen and Singal 2003), vehicle emissions (Marr and Harley 2002), atmospheric dynamics (Atkinson-Palombo et al. 2006), and medical outcomes (Albright et al. 2009; Spiers and Guntheroth 1999), only Mao (2011) has explored the effect of weekends on the spread of influenza, either in a seasonal or novel strain epidemic. With workplaces, school, and many other locations of higher transmission closed, one may expect influenza transmission (or other infectious diseases transmitted via casual contact) to decrease during weekends. Indeed, some studies have shown that influenza transmission decreases over holidays as well as weekends (Cauchemez et al. 2008; Hens et al. 2009; Eames et al. 2010, 2011, 2012).

Mao (2011) used an agent-based model (ABM) with many design features similar to the one used for this study. However, the study region is dissimilar, with a much smaller study population of just under 1 million, while the Chicago area population is over 6 million. Mao used the model to assess a number of temporary school closing options that included closing for more than 2 days a week. We focused on the option of adding one more day to the 2-day weekend, not distinguishing between a Friday or a Monday closure, because we consider this option as a practical alternative to temporary school closure. Based on the school closure response to the 2009 H1N1 epidemic in Pennsylvania school officials are reluctant to close schools longer than one or 2 weeks. Our study also extends the results reported in Mao (2011) by providing an economic analysis of the 3-day weekend policy and assessing how long to maintain the policy before reverting back to the 2-day status quo.

School closings reduce student contacts, an intervention strategy often considered during epidemics. Araz et al. (2012) report that school closures can significantly reduce the total number of influenza cases. However, Lee et al. (2010c) demonstrated schools may need to be closed for lengthy periods (e.g., throughout the duration of the epidemic) to be effective. In fact, school closures that do not extend throughout the course of an epidemic may actually worsen it. Reopening schools refuels the epidemic by releasing a large number of susceptible children back into the transmission pool. Lempel et al. (2009) characterized the substantial financial cost of closing schools to mitigate an epidemic. Brown et al. (2011) determined that if school closure had been widely used as a mitigation strategy for the 2009 H1N1 epidemic in Pennsylvania, the cost may have far outweighed the potential cost-savings from reducing the number influenza cases.

These observations raise the possibility that extending the weekend from 2 to 3 days could be a cost-effective control strategy and may not drastically effect society as lengthy school closures do. But, because many existing published influenza models treat each simulation day as a weekday and do not distinguish weekends, this effect on influenza transmission has not been fully explored.

One argument for not considering extended weekends as an important mechanism and not including it in models of influenza spread is the assumption that students engage in compensatory behaviors on the weekends that make up for the school contact (Cauchemez et al. 2008). The compensatory behaviors referenced include additional trips to the mall, the grocery store, etc., and “hanging out” with friends.

A survey by McCaw et al. (2010) indicated that an individual has two to four more personal contacts during weekends than weekdays. However, a study of university students found that individual contacts per day declined by 27 % during weekends (Edmunds et al. 2006). This is clearly a parameter that requires sensitivity investigation.

This study uses an ABM of the Chicago, Illinois metropolitan area to assess if including weekend effects in simulation models of infectious disease transmissions matters. It also addresses whether including weekend effects is offset by increases in weekend contact patterns. Finally, it assesses whether extending the weekend to 3 days has a sufficient effect on disease transmission to warrant support as an intervention mechanism. The influenza natural history parameters used in this model were developed by Ferguson et al. (2005). They characterize how infected persons pass from pathogen exposure to the appearance of symptoms within mean 1.48 days infection, followed by an infectious period that produces a generation time of 2.6 days, after which they recover and are immune to repeated infection or die. The U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) reported the generation time of H1N1 2009 influenza as 2.0–3.1 days (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2009).

Also, Cauchemez et al. (2009) report the mean time between the onset of symptoms in a case patient and the onset of symptoms in the household contacts infected by that individual to be 2.6 days. To accommodate prior immunity to H1N1, we used age-specific estimates from a cross-sectional serological study in England (Miller et al. 2010).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Chicago: Cook and DuPage county study area

Chicago is the county seat of Cook county, with a small portion extending into DuPage county. Collectively, the two counties contain a metropolitan area of 6,184,869 residents, with adjoining suburban and rural areas. The region’s population is 78 % urban white and workplace density centers exist in both counties (U.S. Census Bureau 2009).

A synthetic agent database was developed to represent the human population of the study region (Wheaton et al. 2009). In this ABM, agents represent individual persons. Complex, large-scale social systems are simulated by assigning behaviors and activities to agents within the population, then allowing the agents to interact with each other and the environment.

The synthesized agent database was adapted and extended from a method originally developed by Beckman et al. (1996) that employs the U.S. Census Bureau’s Public Use Microdata files and census-aggregated data to generate synthesized, geospatially explicit human agents that represent actual populations when aggregated (Wheaton et al. 2009). Each agent is assigned to a household with other agents. Similarly, agents are individually assigned to schools and workplaces with other agents. After generating a U.S. population of 105,480,101 households containing 273,624,650 people (2000 Census), data for Cook and DuPage counties was extracted.

2.2 Model description

In each individual agent, disease progresses through an underlying susceptible-exposed-infectious-recovered (SEIR) disease model. At the start of each simulation run, individuals who have already been vaccinated or infected (recently or remotely) are classified as recovered (R), and therefore immune to, subsequent infection. An H1N1 vaccine was not available at the start of the epidemic so we assumed no one in the simulation was protected except those subjects with prior immunity. All other individuals were initially susceptible (S) to influenza. At the start of each simulation run, 100 randomly chosen agents were infected to generate the epidemic. Every susceptible individual who contacted an infectious individual had a probability of contracting influenza (Longini et al. 2005; Halloran et al. 2008). Newly infected agents then progressively move into the exposed (E) state, in which they remain for the duration of the disease’s incubation period. They then move to the infectious (I) state, in which the agent can infect others. In the simulation, the length of time in a given state is determined by randomly selecting variants from an incubation period and from infectious period distributions.

Two-thirds of infectious patients manifest symptoms; the remaining third are asymptomatic but are able to transmit disease [Writing Committee of the WHO Consultation on Clinical Aspects of Epidemic (H1N1) 2009]. The presence of symptoms is associated with higher transmissibility; typically, symptomatic infections are assumed to be twice as infectious as asymptomatic infections. However, one recent study estimated the infectious rate to be 3–12 times higher for symptomatic infections (Van Kerckhove et al. 2013). Our model assumes the lower value, so our symptomatic agents are three times as infectious as asymptomatic agents. According to data from the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2013, 2014), 34 % of the workforce works on weekends and 63 % of the adult population works. Because a portion of the 63 % working population is unemployed, we estimated that 21 % of all adults work on weekends. Thus, our model also assumes that 21 % of randomly selected adults work on weekends (consistent with Bonne 2003) and that 90 % of sick students and workers, or agents with symptomatic disease, remain at home without generating community-level contacts unless they see a doctor. Previous studies explore the lower rates of withdrawal to the home (Cooley et al. 2010, 2011, Lee et al. 2010a, 2010b; Rothberg and Rose 2005). The sensitivity of changes to these assumptions and their effect on simulated epidemics has been examined (Cooley et al. 2011). However, a recent study reported significantly fewer contacts among people with influenza symptoms; consequently, this study assumes the higher withdrawal rate resulting in reduced social mixing patterns (Van Kerckhove et al. 2013). The ABM used for this study also tracks features similar to those defined by Ferguson et al. (2006) and Germann et al. (2006), including age, sex, occupation, household location, household membership, school assignment of students and teachers, work location assignment of employed adults, work status as employed or unemployed, and disease status.

Our model assigned 1,185,062 school-aged children to 2103 school locations. The ArcGIS Business Analyst (ESRI, Redlands, CA) identified 162,648 businesses with total employment of 2,504,324 in Cook and DuPage counties. We assigned 2,584,324 working adults to one of the workplaces by using the census 2000 special tabulation: census tract of work by census tract of residence (STP 64) database. This later data source supports an accurate representation of commuting patterns. Also, based on prior work (Cooley et al. 2011), we determined that incorporating public transportation into our model introduced extra complexity while contributing little to influenza transmission levels. We therefore decided not to model transportation; thus, agents move instantaneously between activities.

A fuller discussion of the assignment process is described elsewhere (Cajka et al. 2010).

Note that alternative models to our social network model exist that are driven by activity networks compiled from surveys of activities that occur throughout the day on a sample of representative persons. The sample traits are then synthesized and assigned to the entire simulation population. This approach legitimately argues that they represent a wider and more heterogeneous distribution of agent behaviors. However, these attributes are undermined because they also represent all days in a week as identical (our models distinguish between weekdays and weekends).

Furthermore, the Halloran et al. study (2008), compared three models of the Chicago metropolitan area. One of these (Episims) was an activity-based network model. The other two were social network models. While each generated different infection curves, all of the models ranked the value of the simulated intervention policies in the same order. By this policy evaluation criteria, there was no difference between the two model approaches.

2.3 Model parameters and social network structure

The ABM infection transmission probabilities are a product of the probability of daily contacts of sufficient duration and, given a contact occurs, the probability of transmission. The contact probabilities (Table 1) were obtained from two related studies by Longini et al. (2005) and Germann et al. (2006). Both sources are derived from data on the 1957–1958 Asian influenza epidemic. The contact probabilities in Table 1 depend on the age of both the infectious and susceptible persons and represent the likelihood of these two individuals having a daily contact of sufficient duration and closeness to transmit influenza.

Table 1 Agent-based model and daily person-to-person contact parameter values within contact groups

The 6,184,869 individuals who are the region’s household population are the model’s circulating agents. The agents’ locations are tracked and those who interact with other agents in close proximity are potential flu transmitters. For example, an agent interacts daily with family members; nonfamily members sharing a household interact with each other less than daily but at least four times a week.

Also, each student or worker also has a random probability of interacting with people in other classrooms or offices. There are 2103 schools in the region educating 1,185,062 students, and there are 162,245 workplaces (including hospitals and clinics) employing 2,584,324 workers. In schools and workplaces, each individual contacts the same set of persons each day, although the number of contacts vary by day. Workers in small firms have repeated contacts with the same people daily and all agents, including students, interact in the community daily including weekends, with student–community interactions increasing on weekends.

2.4 Model calibration

We calibrated our model using data from the H1N1 epidemic as reported by Cauchemez et al. (2009). These data are summarized in Table 2 and were based on an analysis of a pandemic influenza outbreak in an elementary school that spread to a rural community.

Table 2 Quantification of H1N1 transmission occurrences

Calibration involved targeting an epidemic with a 16 % illness infection attack rate (AR). This is based on the rate reported by Riley et al. (2011) for the 2009 H1N1 epidemic in Hong Kong.

The number of transmissible contacts are estimated endogenously to reproduce the proportion of infections that occur by place i.e. the observed distributions of infections by place represented in Table 2. This process accounts for prior immunity by age and estimates the following contact rates:

  • students at school,

  • students at schools within classrooms,

  • adults in the workplace,

  • students in the community,

  • adults in the community

  • adult to adult in the household,

  • child to child in the household,

  • adult to child in the household, and

  • child to adult in the household.

The endogenously estimated daily contact rates are placed into a standard SEIR framework used to describe infectious disease models. The social network assumptions are straightforward. Students mix with other students assigned to their school as well as their classroom. Workers mix with workers assigned to their workplace. Household dwellers mix with other persons living within the household and everyone mixes with other people in the community such that the probability of a contact is inversely related to the distance between the infected person’s household location and a random number of persons living within the simulated region (also estimated endogenously).

Under the above constraints, the estimated contact patterns are designed to reproduce an epidemic similar to the H1N1 flu epidemic of 2009, with a sickness (symptomatic) attack rate of 16 %, a total infection (asymptomatic plus symptomatic) rate of 24 %, and a basic reproductive rate (R0) of 1.2.2.5 FluEcon: influenza economic model.

The number of symptomatic cases and the number of people missing work or school due to illness or intervention generated by our ABM fed into FluEcon, our Monte Carlo economic simulation model, which was developed in microsoft excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA) with the crystal ball add-in (Oracle, Redwood City, CA), to translate influenza cases into direct health care costs and indirect costs (i.e., lost productivity from illness and mortality) as detailed in previous publications (Brown et al. 2011; Lee et al. 2012a, 2012b, 2015). Each symptomatic individual had probabilities of seeking outpatient treatment, being hospitalized, or dying from influenza (Molinari et al. 2007). Each of these outcomes were age-specific and were associated with a corresponding cost; thus, the outcome of each case determined the costs accrued. Outpatient visit and hospitalization costs came from nationally representative databases (Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services 2011; Thomson Healthcare 2008 U.S. Department of Health and Human Services 2011).

Symptomatic cases resulted in lost productivity during the duration of their symptoms (if not hospitalized) or hospitalization. The median hourly wage for all occupations from the Department of Labor served as proxies for productivity losses and assumed an 8-h workday (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics 2010). Agents that died incurred productivity losses representing the net present value of that person’s remaining lifetime earnings, based on their life expectancy as derived from the human mortality database (University of California, Berkeley and Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research 2011). A 3 % discount rate converted all costs into 2014 values.

Total influenza-related costs included direct and indirect costs, including missed work, school, and sick days due to influenza. For intervention costs, we assumed that all adults missing work due to closure incurred a day of lost productivity and there was no possibility of telecommuting. Because businesses and schools were closed on the same day, we assumed a child missing school would have a parent at home or would be cared for by a neighbor or relative, resulting in no additional productivity losses.

Mitchell County, North Carolina residents reported this experience responding to a 2-week school closure brought on by an influenza B epidemic in fall 2006. According to a study, “For parents, the outbreak and school closings meant missing work, getting relatives to watch children, even taking well youngsters to work” (White and Young 2006, FluTrackers.com).

2.5 Varying weekend length

The set of experiments on weekends had two objectives. The first was to determine whether including weekends makes a difference in simulation models of infectious disease transmissions. The second was to assess if extending the weekend from 2 to 3 days can mitigate the severity of an epidemic.

Every day, all agents, including students, potentially interact with each other in the community, although with a fairly low probability of transmitting the virus. On weekends, student agents do not go to school but have an undetermined number of community interactions. Our model assumed that students would have (1) no extra weekend contacts, (2) 25 % more community weekend contacts than community weekday contacts, or (3) 100 % more community weekend contacts than community weekday contacts. Assumption 2 is more consistent with estimates derived from information on French student vacationers (Cauchemez et al. 2008).

2.6 Simulation scenarios

We used several simulation scenarios to produce comparisons to investigate the study objectives. For all scenarios, the effect of prior immunity to the H1N1 pathogen protects older adults but not school-aged children (Miller et al. 2010). We present and compare six specific scenarios in Table 3. Scenario WE-0 assumes every day of the week has the same contact profile, similar to assumptions in many models.

Table 3 Summary of simulated scenarios

We used three 2-day weekend scenarios. Scenario WE-2a assumes that on weekdays children interact with other children and adults at malls, various shopping venues, and social events. On weekends, these children stay home and only interact within the household. This unlikely scenario would be a strict quarantine on weekends. Scenario WE-2b assumes that children interact with other students and adults on weekdays and on weekend days students increase their weekday contacts by 25 %. These estimates are based on a French study that assumes that during holidays, transmission does not occur in schools but in other places such as the household and the community (Cauchemez et al. 2009). We assume that the change in patterns of contact during holidays is similar to changes between weekdays and weekends. The increase suggested by the French study was a 50 % larger contact rate during holidays. We assume a smaller 25 % increase per day (for 2 days). Finally, scenario WE-2c increases the weekend contacts by 100 % above the baseline—beyond what we believe to be a realistic assumption, but one that supports a simple sensitivity analysis by bracketing the “true” compensatory contact behavior pattern.

The other two scenarios assume 3-day weekends. Scenario WE-3a simulates a 3-day weekend run that assumes that after a specific threshold number of cases is reached (100,000 cases was selected arbitrarily), school operations then move to a 4-day school attendance week and that this pattern extended to the end of the epidemic period. Scenario WE-3b used the same threshold (100,000 cases) but only operates this policy for 60 days. During the extended weekend period, there is no difference in contact patterns except on that extra weekend day. For Scenarios WE-3a and WE-3b, on the third weekend day there are no school contacts but there is a 25 % increase in community contacts.

3 Results

Figures 1 through 4 and Table 4 describe the comparisons among the scenarios listed in Table 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Comparison between 2-day weekend (WE-2a) and 0-day weekend (WE-0) models

Table 4 A comparison of 3-day weekend intervention scenarios versus a 2-day baseline scenario

As shown in Fig. 1, both the WE-2a and WE-0 scenarios display the infection curve of total infections by day and each scenario is calibrated to the same criteria (Table 2) with an illness attack rate of 16 %. The scenarios without a weekend (WE-0) are smoother and are left-shifted, in contrast to the 2-day weekend without compensatory behavior scenario (WE-2a). Also, the no-weekend model has a shorter epidemic period (from 175 to 168 days), has a similar maximum peak daily infection (41,348 versus 38,407), and an earlier peak case incidence day (49 vs. 54).

The four scenarios shown in Fig. 2 are calibrated to the criteria defined in Table 2 and accordingly the area under each curve records nearly identical infection prevalence. The 2-day weekend scenarios with varying rates of compensatory behavior are all right-shifted relative to the no-weekend scenario (WE-0). The scenario with a 100 % increase of compensatory contacts on weekends (WE-2c) approximates most closely the shape of the no-weekend scenario attack rate curve. Because the results of other weekend studies support a lower rate of compensatory behavior, we felt that adding an average of 25 % increases in compensatory community contacts of students on weekends was the most realistic with respect to the supporting data. We characterize this scenario as the baseline scenario (WE-2b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Compensatory behavior analysis

Figure 3 compares a 2-day weekend baseline scenario (WE-2b) with a 3-day weekend scenario that operates for only 60 days (WE-3b). The WE-2b curve shows that initiating an extended 3-day weekend on day 28 of the epidemic (after reaching 100,000 cases), has a noticeable effect on the peak incidence rate and the overall attack rate. Maintaining a 3-day weekend policy for an arbitrary 60-day period substantially reduces (by more than half) both the peak and overall serological attack rate (24.0–16.5 %). This demonstrates the potential benefit of the 3-day weekend as a containment strategy in contrast to full school closure, but leaves the cost issue unresolved.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Effect of adding a third day to the weekend

Figure 4 shows a comparison of both 3-day weekend scenarios. WE-3a initiates the 3-day weekend on day 28 and sustains it to the end of the epidemic period; WE-3b also initiates the 3-day weekend on day 28 but sustains it for 60 days before reverting back to a 2-day weekend. Figure 4 suggests there is little difference between the two scenarios and suggests that to be cost-effective, identifying a second threshold that causes the 3-day weekend intervention to revert to the status quo is important.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effects of ending the 3-day weekend at different points in the epidemic

To demonstrate the economic difference between the two scenarios, we performed an economic analysis of the two 3-day weekend scenarios in contrast to the baseline no-intervention scenario (WE-2b) and the results are shown in Table 4. Based on the model assumptions reported, the mean estimated savings to society for implementing a 3-day weekend intervention in a Chicago-like city ranges from $332.9 million to $248.6 million. In other words, closing schools for 9 Mondays during the peak period of a flu pandemic in a city similar to Chicago could possibly prevent up to $248.6 million in losses. The range of the estimates is based on the length of the intervention and do not reflect any differences in the educational benefit attributed to a 5- versus a 4-day work/school week. These cost estimates in Table 4 include mean and 95 confidence intervals for the cost estimates generated by FluEcon. The standard errors due to the Monte Carlo process are small resulting in small 95 confidence intervals and are not shown.

4 Discussion

Using an epidemic model that has been calibrated to reproduce the patterns of the H1N1 epidemic of 2009, this study assessed the implications of representing weekend patterns in influenza models. The results indicate that leaving weekend behaviors out of a model shortens the predicted epidemic period unless unrealistically high rates of compensatory community contacts (with school friends, mall encounters, etc.) are also represented. With the exception of higher school infection rates and a leftward shift in the epidemic, there is little difference between the 0 weekend and the 2-day weekend models. This result is directly attributed to a high rate of withdrawal (90 %) to the home by symptomatic individuals.

Extending the weekend from 2 to 3 days was simulated to assess if 3-day weekends were an effective influenza intervention strategy. The results suggest that extended weekends could have a significant effect on reducing peak attack rates and societal costs. It is also possible that reducing peak attack rates could extend the epidemic period. Although the model does not incorporate seasonal effects, it may overstate the overall infection rate, which would diminish as a consequence of seasonal effects, especially during the later stages of the epidemic period.

Overall, the evidence from our simulations suggests that using a 3-day weekend as an intervention strategy could be effective for mild epidemics similar in severity to the H1N1 epidemic of 2009 and with a concentration in school-aged children. Such an intervention would also be far less detrimental to the educational process than sustained closure because students and teachers could maintain contact throughout the epidemic period. In addition, a 4-day week might easily be accommodated by many types of businesses and schools; in fact, 21 states currently support 4-day school weeks. (National Conference of State Legislators 2013). There certainly could be substantial costs to closing schools for this additional day, especially in urban areas. Two separate studies reported that closing schools completely for a sustained period during the 2009 H1N1 epidemic could have resulted in substantial societal costs because the lost productivity and child care costs could have far outweighed the savings created by preventing influenza cases (Brown et al. 2011; Lempel et al. 2009). However, our economic analysis suggests the opposite would occur if schools are closed only 1 day per week for a 2-month period. The lower costs and subsequent savings associated with nine consecutive 3-day weekends during the critical 2-month period of the H1N1 influenza season would more than compensate the costs of school and workplace closure.