Abstract
Mitigating climate change will require the participation of citizens and consumers. A recent study in Climatic Change by Obradovich and Guenther reported that framing responsibility for climate change in terms of collective—as opposed to personal—behaviors generated greater donations to environmental groups as well as higher self-reported levels of willingness to adopt environmentally-friendly behaviors. As East Asia is the leading emitter of greenhouse gases globally, these findings are or clear relevance to the region. Nonetheless, recent findings in cultural psychology suggest that this framing intervention may not have the same results in an East Asian cultural context. We therefore sought to determine whether these findings could be replicated in East Asia. For this study, 2085 university students in Taiwan were randomly assigned to receive either a collective responsibility priming task, a personal responsibility priming task, or a daily routine priming task (control). They were then given the opportunity to donate to a climate-related cause and asked to report on their likelihood of changing their personal behaviors to reduce carbon emissions. Participants in the collective and personal conditions donated significantly more than those in the control condition and those in the personal responsibility condition reported significantly lower probabilities of changing their behaviors than those in both the control and collective responsibility conditions. Our study provides a partial replication with a different demographic group and in a different cultural setting, strengthening the argument for collective responsibility framing and setting the stage for research into practical implementations.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Arnett JJ (2008) The neglected 95%: why American psychology needs to become less American. Am Psychol 63(7):602–614. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602
Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS (2012) Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's mechanical Turk. Polit Anal 20(3):351–368
Bernauer T, McGrath LF (2016) Simple reframing unlikely to boost public support for climate policy. Nat Clim Chang. https://doi.org/10.1038/NCLIMATE2948
Bissing-Olson MJ, Fielding KS, Iyer A (2016) Experiences of pride, not guilt, predict pro-environmental behavior when pro-environmental descriptive norms are more positive. J Environ Psychol 45:145–153
Blanken I, van de Ven N, Zeelenberg M, Meijers MH (2014) Three attempts to replicate the moral licensing effect. Soc Psychol 45(3):232
Boden TA, Marland G, and Andres RJ (2017) National CO2 emissions from fossil-fuel burning, cement manufacture, and gas flaring: 1751–2014, Carbon Dioxide Information Analysis Center, Oak Ridge National Laboratory, U.S. Department of Energy, doi https://doi.org/10.3334/CDIAC/00001_V2017
Brick C, Lai CK (2018) Explicit (but not implicit) environmentalist identity predicts pro-environmental behavior and policy preferences. J Environ Psychol 58:8–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.07.003
Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD (2011) Amazon's mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 6(1):3–5
Burck J, Marten F, Bals C (2015) The climate change performance index: results 2016. Germanwatch, Berlin
Burck J, Marten F, Bals C, Hohne N (2017) The climate change performance index: results 2018. Germanwatch, Berlin
Chen C, Lee SY, Stevenson HW (1995) Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales among east Asian and north American students. Psychol Sci 6:170–175
Cooper H, Patall EA (2009) The relative benefits of meta-analysis conducted with individual participant data versus aggregated data. Psychol Methods 14(2):165
Di Giusto B, Lavallee JP, Yu TY (2018) Towards an east Asian model of climate change awareness: a questionnaire study among university students in Taiwan. PLoS One 13(10):e0206298
Economy Watch (2016) Implied PPP Conversion Rate Data for Year 2016, All Countries. Retrieved from http://www.economywatch.com/economic-statistics/economic-indicators/Implied_PPP_Conversion_Rate/#yearListing
EIA (2013) International Energy Outlook 2013. US Energy Information Administration (EIA)
Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci 33(2–3):61–83. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
IBM Corp Released. (2011). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 20.0. Armonk: IBM Corp
IPCC, 2018: Summary for policymakers. In: Global Warming of 1.5°C. An IPCC Special Report. World Meteorological Organization, Geneva, Switzerland. https://www.ipcc.ch/sr15/chapter/summary-for-policy-makers/. Accessed Dec 2018
Johnson T, Kulesa P, Cho YI, Shavitt S (2005) The relation between culture and response styles: evidence from 19 countries. J Cross-Cult Psychol 36(2):264–277
Kitayama S, Markus HR, Matsumoto H, Norasakkunkit V (1997) Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. J Pers Soc Psychol 72(6):1245
Kitayama S, Mesquita B, Karasawa M (2006) Cultural affordances and emotional experience: socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. J Pers Soc Psychol 91(5):890
Kitayama S, Park J (2013) Culture and the self: implications for consumer behavior. In: Ruvio AA, Belk RW (eds) The Routledge companion to identity and consumption. Routledge
Lee JW, Jones PS, Mineyama Y, Zhang XE (2002) Cultural differences in responses to a Likert scale. Res Nurs Health 25(4):295–296
Li, N, Su, LY (2018) Message framing and climate change communication: a meta-analytical review. J Appl Commun, 102(3), doi:https://doi.org/10.4148/1051-0834.2189
Markus HR, Kitayama S (1991) Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev 98(2):224–253
Markus HR, Kitayama S (1994) A collective fear of the collective: implications for selves and theories of selves. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 20(5):568–579
Mascolo MF, Fischer KW, Li J (2003) Dynamic development of component systems in emotions: pride, shame and guilt in China and the United States. In: Davidson RJ, Schere KR, Goldsmith HH (eds) Handbook of affective sciences. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 375–408
McNeill BJ, Pauker SG, Sox H, Tversky A (1982) On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. N Engl J Med 306(2):1259–1262
Merritt AC, Effron DA, Monin B (2010) Moral self-licensing: when being good frees us to be bad. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 4(5):344–357
Mesquita B, Karasawa M (2004) Self-conscious emotions as dynamic cultural processes. Psychol Inq 15(2):161–166
Obradovich N, Guenther SM (2016) Collective responsibility amplifies mitigation behaviors. Clim Chang 137(1–2):307–319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-016-1670-9
Riley RD, Lambert PC, Abo-Zaid G (2010) Meta-analysis of individual participant data: rationale, conduct, and reporting. Bmj 340:c221
R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria URL http://www.R-project.org/
Schneider CR, Zaval L, Weber EU, Markowitz EM (2017) The influence of anticipated pride and guilt on pro-environmental decision making. PLoS One 12(11):e0188781. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0188781
Simonsohn U (2015) Small telescopes: detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychol Sci 26(5):559–569
Stipek D (1998) Differences between Americans and Chinese in the circumstances evoking pride, shame, and guilt. J Cross-Cult Psychol 29(5):616–629
Tauber S, van Zomeren M, Kutlaca M (2015) Should the moral core of climate issues be emphasized or downplayed in public discourse? Three ways to successfully manage the double-edged sword of moral communication. Clim Chang 130(3):453–464
Thomas D, Radji S, Benedetti A (2014) Systematic review of methods for individual patient data meta-analysis with binary outcomes. BMC Med Res Methodol 14(1):79
Tracy JL, Robins RW (2004) Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model. Psychol Inq 15(2):103–125
Triandis HC, Carnevale P, Gelfand M, Robert C, Wasti SA, Probst T, Kashima ES, Dragonas T, Chan D, Chen XP, Kim U (2001) Culture and deception in business negotiations: a multilevel analysis. Int J Cross-cult Manag 4(1):73–90
Trope Y, Liberman N (2010) Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol Rev 117(2):440
Truelove HB, Carrico AR, Weber EU, Raimi KT, Vandenbergh MP (2014) Positive and negative spillover of pro-environmental behavior: an integrative review and theoretical framework. Glob Environ Chang 29:127–138
Tversky A, Kahneman D (1981) The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211(4481):453–458
UNEP (2018) The emissions gap report 2018. United Nations Environment Programme, Nairobi http://wedocs.unep.org/bitstream/handle/20.500.11822/26895/EGR2018_FullReport_EN.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y. Accessed Dec 2018
Vicente-Molina MA, Fernández-Sainz A, Izagirre-Olaizola J (2018) Does gender make a difference in pro-environmental behavior? The case of the Basque Country University students. J Clean Prod 176:89–98
Wachholz S, Artz N, Chene D (2014) Warming to the idea: university students' knowledge and attitudes about climate change. Int J Sust Higher Ed 15(2):128–141. https://doi.org/10.1108/IJSHE-03-2012-0025
Wallbott HG, Scherer KR (1995) Cultural determinants in experiencing shame and guilt. In: Tangney JP, Fischer KW (eds) Self-conscious emotions. The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. Guilford, New York, pp 465–487
White H (1980) A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator and a direct test for heteroskedasticity. Journal of the Econometric Society, Econometrica, pp 817–838
Xiao C, McCright AM (2015) Gender differences in environmental concern: revisiting the institutional trust hypothesis in the USA. Environ Behav 47(1):17e37
Zeileis A (2004) Econometric computing with HC and HAC covariance matrix estimators. J Stat Softw 11(10):1–17 URL http://www.jstatsoft.org/v11/i10/
Acknowledgments
We are deeply grateful for the constructive comments and suggestions offered by the anonymous reviewers of the earlier versions of this manuscript.
Funding
Support for this study was from Taiwan’s Ministry of Science and Technology (grant number 104-2410-H-130-020).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
ESM 1
(DOCX 37 kb).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lavallee, J.P., Di Giusto, B. & Yu, TY. Collective responsibility framing also leads to mitigation behavior in East Asia: a replication study in Taiwan. Climatic Change 153, 423–438 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-019-02402-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-019-02402-z