Abstract
Survey researchers often treat self-assessed understanding of climate change as a rough proxy for knowledge, which might affect what people believe about this topic. Self-assessments can be unrealistically high, however, and correlated with politics, so they deserve study in their own right. Turning the usual perspective around to view self-assessed understanding as dependent variable, problematically related to actual knowledge, casts self-assessments in a new light. Analysis of a 2016 US survey that carried a five-item test of very basic, belief-neutral but climate-relevant knowledge (such as knowing about the location of North and South Poles) finds that, at any given level of knowledge, people saying they “understand a great deal” about climate change are more likely to be older, college-educated, and male. Self-assessed understanding exhibits a U-shaped political pattern: highest among liberals and the most conservative, but lowest among moderate conservatives. Among liberal and middle-of-the-road respondents, self-assessed understanding of climate change is positively related to knowledge. Among the most conservative, however, understanding is unrelated or even negatively related to knowledge. For that group in particular, high self-assessed understanding reflects confidence in political views, rather than knowledge about the physical world.
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Weights take into account the regional population; number of adults and telephone numbers within households; and respondent age, sex, and race. Response rates in the August and November/December stages of the POLES survey were 27–30% for Alaska, and 15–24% for the other 49 states, as calculated by AAPOR 2016 definition 4.
Similar U-shaped relationships between political identity and high self-assessed understanding of climate change can also be found in many other national or regional survey datasets including those described by Hamilton (2012), Hamilton and Saito (2015), or Hamilton et al. (2016a, 2018a, 2018b). In each of these datasets as in Figure 1d, non-Tea Party Republicans or moderate conservatives express the least confidence in their understanding of climate change.
Principal components analysis (not shown) confirms that these five items load on the first component with roughly similar weights, so a coefficient-weighted knowledge index behaves no differently (r = 0.996) from the simple additive index.
For straightforward interpretation in terms of overconfidence, understanding is here coded as 1 for “a great deal” and 0 otherwise. Alternative specifications using ordinal codes (understanding coded from 1 “nothing” to 4 “a great deal”), estimated by probability-weighted ordered logit regression, yield generally similar conclusions. In the ordinal versions of models 1 and 3, the same predictors have significant effects, with the same signs, as their counterparts in Table 2. The knowledge × party or knowledge × ideology interaction terms are, if anything, slightly stronger in these two ordinal versions. In the ordinal version of model 2, the education effect is stronger but the knowledge × ideology interaction slightly weaker, falling short of statistical significance (p = 0.09), although with the same sign. Otherwise, the significance as well as signs of coefficients are the same for ordinal and dichotomous versions of model 2.
Nor are there significant election × party or election × ideology interactions, which were tested in alternative versions of models 1 and 2 (not shown). Previous analyses looking at different variables on the POLES surveys likewise found little difference between pre- and post-election responses (Hamilton 2017, 2018b).
Because party and ideology interact with knowledge, their main effects in Table 2 describe the effect of party or ideology when knowledge = 0, that is, when no questions are answered correctly.
References
AAPOR (2016). Standard definitions: Final disposition of case codes and outcome rates for surveys (2016 revision). T. W. Smith, Ed., American Association for Public Opinion Research
Bolin JL, Hamilton LC (2018) The news you choose: news media preferences amplify views on climate change. Environ Politics. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2018.1423909
Boutyline A, Willer R (2017) The social structure of political echo chambers: variation in ideological homophily in online networks. Polit Psychol 38:551–569
Bronen R (2009) Forced migration of Alaskan indigenous communities due to climate change: creating a human rights response, pp. 68–73 in Oliver-Smith A, Shen X (eds.) Linking Environmental Change, Migration and Social Vulnerability. Bonn: UNU Institute for Environment and Human Security
Brulle RJ, Carmichael J, Jenkins JC (2012) Shifting public opinion on climate change: an empirical assessment of factors influencing concern over climate change in the U.S., 2002–2010. Clim Chang 114:169–188
Bullock JG (2011) Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. Am Political Sci Review 105:496–515
Carmichael JT, Brulle RJ (2017) Elite cues, media coverage, and public concern: an integrated path analysis of public opinion on climate change, 2001–2013. Environ Politics 26(2):232–252. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2016.1263433
Carmichael JT, Brulle RJ, Huxster JK (2017) The great divide: understanding the role of media and other drivers of the partisan divide in public concern over climate change in the USA, 2001–2014. Clim Chang 141:599–612. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-017-1908-1
Cook J, Oreskes N, Doran PT, Anderegg WRL, Verheggen B, Maibach EW, Carlton JS, Lewandowsky S, Skuce AG, Green SA, Nuccitelli D, Jacobs P, Richardson M, Winkler B, Painting R, Rice K (2016) Consensus on consensus: a synthesis of consensus estimates on human-caused global warming. Environ Res Lett 11(4). https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/11/4/048002
Cook J, Lewandowsky S, Ecker UKH (2017) Neutralizing misinformation through inoculation: exposing misleading argumentation techniques reduces their influence. PLoS One. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175799
Corner A, Whitmarsh L, Xenias D (2012) Uncertainty, scepticism and attitudes towards climate change: biased assimilation and attitude polarisation. Clim Chang 114(3–4):463–478
Darmofal D (2005) Elite cues and citizen disagreement with expert opinion. Political Res Quarterly 58(3):381–395
Ding D, Maibach EW, Zhao X, Roser-Renouf C, Leiserowitz A (2011) Support for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific agreement. Nat Clim Chang 1:462–466. https://doi.org/10.1038/NCLIMATE1295
Drummond C, Fischhoff B (2017) Individuals with greater science literacy and education have more polarized beliefs on controversial science topics. Proc Natl Acad Sci. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704882114
Dunlap RE, McCright AM (2015) Challenging climate change: the denial countermovement. Pp. 300–332 in Dunlap RE, Brulle RJ (eds), Climate Change and Society: Sociological Perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press
Dunlap RE, McCright AM, Yarosh JH (2016) The political divide on climate change: partisan polarization widens in the U.S. Environment 58(5):4–23. https://doi.org/10.1080/00139157.2016.1208995
Ehret PJ, Sparks A, Sherman D (2017) Support for environmental protection: an integration of ideological-consistency and information-deficit models. Environ Politics 26(2):253–277. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2016.1256960
Frimer JA, Skitka LJ, Motyl M (2017) Liberals and conservatives are similarly motivated to avoid exposure to one another’s opinions. J Exp Soc Psychol 72:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.003
Gauchat G (2012) Politicization of science in the public sphere: a study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. Am Sociol Rev 77:167–187. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122412438225
Guess A, Nyhan B, Reifler J (2018) Selective exposure to misinformation: evidence from the consumption of fake news during the 2016 U.S. presidential campaign. Working paper available at: https://t.co/lUN71y3DhT
Hamilton LC (2008) Who cares about polar regions? Results from a survey of U.S. public opinion. Arct Antarct Alp Res 40(4):671–678
Hamilton LC (2011) Education, politics and opinions about climate change: evidence for interaction effects. Clim Chang 104:231–242. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-010-9957-8
Hamilton LC (2012) Did the Arctic ice recover? Demographics of true and false climate facts. Weather, Climate, Soc 4(4):236–249. https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-12-00008.1
Hamilton LC (2015a) Polar facts in the age of polarization. Polar Geogr 38(2):89–106. https://doi.org/10.1080/1088937X.2015.1051158
Hamilton LC (2015b) Conservative and liberal views of science: does trust depend on topic? Durham, NH: Carsey School of Public Policy. http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/252/
Hamilton LC (2016a) Public awareness of the scientific consensus on climate. SAGE Open. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244016676296
Hamilton LC (2016b) Where is the North Pole? An election-year survey on global change. Durham, NH: Carsey School of Public Policy. http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/285/
Hamilton LC (2017) Public acceptance of human-caused climate change is gradually rising. Durham, NH: Carsey School of Public Policy. http://scholars.unh.edu/carsey/322/
Hamilton LC, Saito K (2015) A four-party view of U.S. environmental concern. Environ Politics 24(2):212–227. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2014.976485
Hamilton LC, Cutler MJ, Schaefer A (2012) Public knowledge and concern about polar-region warming. Polar Geogr 35(2):155–168. https://doi.org/10.1080/1088937X.2012.684155
Hamilton LC, Hartter J, Lemcke-Stampone M, Moore DW, Safford TG (2015a) Tracking public beliefs about anthropogenic climate change. PLoS One 10(9):e0138208. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138208
Hamilton LC, Hartter J, Saito K (2015b) Trust in scientists on climate change and vaccines. SAGE Open. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244015602752
Hamilton LC, Saito K, Loring PA, Lammers RB, Huntington HP (2016a) Climigration? Population and climate change in Arctic Alaska. Popul Environ 38(2):115–133. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11111-016-0259-6
Hamilton LC, Hartter J, Keim BD, Boag AE, Palace MW, Stevens FR, Ducey MJ (2016b) Wildfire, climate, and perceptions in Northeast Oregon. Reg Environ Chang 16:1819–1832. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10113-015-0914-y
Hamilton LC, Lemcke-Stampone M, Grimm C (2018a) Cold winters warming? Perceptions of climate change in the North Country. Weather, Climate, and Soc. https://doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-18-0020.1
Hamilton LC, Bell E, Hartter J, Salerno JD (2018b) A change in the wind? U.S. public views on renewable energy and climate compared. Energy, Sustainability Soc 8(11). https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2018.1423909
Jost JT (2017) Asymmetries abound: ideological differences in emotion, partisanship, motivated reasoning, social network structure, and political trust. J Consum Psychol 27(4):546–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2017.08.004
Kahan DM (2015) Climate science communication and the measurement problem. Advances Political Psychology 36(1):1–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12244
Kahan DM, Jenkins-Smith H, Braman D (2011) Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. J Risk Res 14(2):147–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
Kahan DM, Peters E, Wittlin M, Slovic P, Ouellette LL, Braman D, Manel G (2012) The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nat Clim Chang 2(10):732–735. https://doi.org/10.1038/NCLIMATE1547
Kay AC, Whitson JA, Gaucher D, Galinsky AD (2009) Compensatory control: achieving order through the mind, our institutions, and the heavens. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 18(5):264–268
Kraft PW, Lodge M, Taber CS (2015) Why people ‘Don’t trust the evidence’: motivated reasoning and scientific beliefs. Annals, American Academy Political Social Sci 658:121–133. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716214554758
Kunda Z (1990) The case for motivated reasoning. Psychol Bull 108(3):480–498
Leiserowitz A, Smith N, Marlon JR (2010) Americans’ Knowledge of Climate Change. (New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication). Available online at http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/ClimateChangeKnowledge2010.pdf
Leiserowitz A, Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Hmielowski JD (2011) Politics & global warming: Democrats, Republicans, Independents, and the Tea Party. New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication. Available online at http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/PoliticsGlobalWarming2011.pdf accessed 12/5/2014
Lewandowsky S, Gignac GE, Vaughan S (2013) The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nat Clim Chang 3:399–404. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1720
McCright AM, Dunlap RE (2011) The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociol Q 52:155–194
Maibach E, Myers T, Leiserowitz A (2014) Climate scientists need to set the record straight: there is a scientific consensus that human-caused climate change is happening. Earth’s Future 2(5):295–298. https://doi.org/10.1002/2013EF000226
Malka A, Krosnick JA, Langer G (2009) The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk Anal 229(5):633–647
Marino E (2015) Fierce climate, sacred ground. University of Alaska Press, Fairbanks
Nadelson L, Jorcyk C, Yang D, Smith MJ, Matson S, Cornell K, Husting V (2014) I just don’t trust them: the development and validation of an assessment instrument to measure trust in science and scientists. Sch Sci Math 114:76–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/ssm.12051
National Science Board (2010) Science and engineering indicators 2010 (Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation). Available online at http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/seind10/pdf/seind10.pdf
Rodriguez CG, Moskowitz JP, Salem RM, Ditto PH (2017) Partisan selective exposure: the role of party, ideology and ideological extremity over time. Translational Issues Psychological Sci 3(3):254–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000121
Roos JM (2014) Measuring science or religion? A measurement analysis of the National Science Foundation sponsored science literacy scale 2006–2010. Public Underst Sci 23(7):797–813. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662512464318
Saad L (2017) Global warming concern at three-decade high in U.S. Gallup News. http://news.gallup.com/poll/206030/global-warming-concern-three-decade-high.aspx
Shao W (2016) Weather, climate, politics, or God? Determinants of American public opinions toward global warming. Environ Politics 26(1):71–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/09644016.2016.1223190
Suldovsky B (2017) The information deficit model and climate change communication. Climate Change Communication. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.301
Taber CS, Lodge M (2006) Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. Am J Polit Sci 50(3):755–769
USACE (2009) Alaska Baseline Erosion Assessment: Study Findings and Technical Report. U.S. Army Corps of Engineers. http://www.poa.usace.army.mil/Portals/34/docs/civilworks/BEA/ AlaskaBaselineErosionAssessmentBEAMainReport.pdf
van der Linden SL, Leiserowitz AA, Feinberg GD, Maibach EW (2015) The scientific consensus on climate change as a gateway belief: experimental evidence. PLoS One 10(2):e0118489. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118489
van der Linden SL, Leiserowitz A, Maibach E (2017a) Scientific agreement can neutralize politicization of facts. Nature Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0259-2
van der Linden S, Leiserowitz A, Rosenthal S, Maibach E (2017b) Inoculating the public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges. https://doi.org/10.1002/gch2.201600008
Washburn AN, Skitka LJ (2017) Science denial across the political divide: liberals and conservatives are similarly motivated to deny attitude-inconsistent science. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617731500
Acknowledgments
Additional climate questions on the Granite State Poll have been supported by the Carsey School of Public Policy and the Sustainability Institute at the University of New Hampshire. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations in this paper are those of the author, and do not necessarily represent the views of the National Science Foundation or other supporting organizations.
Funding
The POLES survey and polar questions on the Granite State Poll were supported through the PoLAR Partnership grant from the National Science Foundation (DUE-1239783).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Hamilton, L.C. Self-assessed understanding of climate change. Climatic Change 151, 349–362 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-018-2305-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-018-2305-0