Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Should the moral core of climate issues be emphasized or downplayed in public discourse? Three ways to successfully manage the double-edged sword of moral communication

  • Published:
Climatic Change Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The main objective of this paper is to identify a serious problem for communicators regarding the framing of climate issues in public discourse, namely that moralizing such an issue can motivate individuals while at the same time defensively lead them to avoid solving the problem. We review recent social-psychological research on moral motivation, concluding that moralization is a double-edged sword: It provides people with a powerful motivation to act for a cause they believe in, yet people often cope with moral threats in defensive ways. Fortunately, recent research also hints at possible solutions of this dilemma of communication. One solution involves the non-moral framing of persuasive messages as a means to avoid defensive responses. Another solution revolves around promoting coping mechanisms that do not reflect defensiveness, such as the promotion of value-driven group identities and the development of moral convictions that increase a sense of agency. Finally, we suggest ways to developing change-oriented moral convictions about climate issues. Our findings are of substantial relevance for scientists and policy makers who aim at stimulating behavioural change (e.g., governments’ commitment to the reduction of GHG emissions).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bamberg S, Möser G (2007) Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J Environ Psychol 27(1):14–25

  • Bauman CW, Skitka LJ (2009) In the mind of the perceiver: Psychological implications of moral conviction. In: Bartels D, Bauman CW, Skitka LJ, Medin D (eds) Moral judgment and decision making. Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, pp 339–362

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender L, Burns SZ, David L, Guggenheim D (2006) An inconvenient truth [motion picture]. Paramount Pictures, United States

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliuc AM, McGarty C, Reynolds K, Muntele D (2007) Opinion‐based group membership as a predictor of commitment to political action. Eur J Soc Psychol 37:19–32. doi:10.1002/ejsp.334

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolderdijk JW, Steg L, Geller ES, Lehman PK, Postmes T (2013) Comparing the effectiveness of monetary versus moral motives in environmental campaigning. Nat Clim Chang 3:413–416. doi:10.1038/NCLIMATE1767

    Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA (1990) A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J Pers Soc Psychol 58:1015–1026. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MJ (2000) Ecological modernisation, environmental knowledge and national character: a preliminary analysis of the Netherlands. Environ Polit 9:77–106. doi:10.1080/09644010008414513

    Google Scholar 

  • Deffke U (2013) Electric mobility - rethinking the car. Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF), Department for Electronic Systems and Electric Mobility. Retrieved from http://www.bmbf.de/pub/electric_mobility_rethinking_the_car.pdf on 23.08.2013

  • Does S, Derks B, Ellemers N (2011) Thou shall not discriminate: how emphasizing moral ideals rather than obligations increases Whites’ support for social equality. J Exp Soc Psychol 47:562–571. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.024

    Google Scholar 

  • Drury J, Reicher S (2009) Collective psychological empowerment as a model of social change: researching crowds and power. J Soc Issues 65:707–725

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg M, Willer R (2013) The moral roots of environmental attitudes. Psychol Sci 24:56–62. doi:10.1177/0956797612449177

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske S, Cuddy A, Glick P (2007) Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends Cogn Sci 11:77–83. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005

    Google Scholar 

  • Good A, Abraham C (2007) Measuring defensive responses to threatening messages: a meta-analysis of measures. Health Psychol Rev 1:208–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt J (2007) The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science 316:998–1002. doi:10.1126/science.1137651

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harth NS, Kessler T, Leach CW (2008) Advantaged group’s emotional reactions to intergroup inequality: the dynamics of pride, guilt, and sympathy. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34:115–129. doi:10.1177/0146167207309193

    Google Scholar 

  • Iyer A, Leach C (2010) Helping disadvantaged out-groups challenge unjust inequality. In Stürmer S, Snyder M (eds) The psychology of prosocial behavior: Group processes, intergroup relations, and helping. Willey-Blackwell, pp 337–353

  • Iyer A, Schmader T, Lickel B (2007) Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: the role of anger, shame, and guilt. Pers Soc Psychol B 33:572–587. doi:10.1177/0146167206297402

    Google Scholar 

  • Klandermans B (1997) The social psychology of protest. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutlaca M, Van Zomeren M, Epstude K (2013) Preaching to the choir or beyond? Effective value-identity framing toward ideologically heterogeneous groups. Unpublished manuscript

  • Leach CW, Snider N, Iyer A (2002) Poisoning the consciences of the fortunate. The experience of relative advantage and support for social equality. In: Walker I, Smiths H (eds) Relative deprivation: Specification, development and integration. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 136–163

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach CW, Iyer A, Pedersen A (2006) Anger and guilt about in-group advantage explain the willingness for political action. Pers Soc Psychol B 32:1232–1245. doi:10.1177/0146167206289729

    Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal H (1970) Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 5: 119–186

  • Lindenberg S, Steg L (2007) Normative, gain and hedonic goal frames guiding environmental behavior. J Soc Issue 63:117–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Lodewijkx HF, Kersten GL, Van Zomeren M (2008) Dual pathways to engage in ‘silent marches’ against violence: moral outrage, moral cleansing and modes of identification. J Community Appl Soc 18:153–167. doi:10.1002/casp.916

    Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz EM, Shariff AF (2012) Climate change and moral judgement. Nat Clim Chang 2:243–247. doi:10.1038/NCLIMATE1378

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni D, van Zomeren M, Cicognani E (2013) The motivating role of right violation, efficacy beliefs and community integration: the case of the Italian water movement. Polit Psychol 23:314–330. doi:10.1002/casp.2123

    Google Scholar 

  • Monin B (2007) Holier than me? Threatening social comparison in the moral domain. Rev Int Psychol Soc 1:53–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan GS, Skitka LJ, Wisneski DC (2010) Moral and religious convictions and intentions to vote in the 2008 presidential election. Anal Soc Issues Public Policy 10:307–320. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2010.01204.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Opotow S (1990) Moral exclusion and injustice: an introduction. J Soc Issues 46:1–20. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb00268.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers RW (1975) A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. TJ Psychol 91:93–114. doi:10.1080/00223980.1975.9915803

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin P (1999) The process of moralization. Psychol Sci 10:218–221. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00139

    Google Scholar 

  • Sassenberg K, Woltin KA (2008) Group-based self-regulation: the effects of regulatory focus. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 19:126–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Skitka LJ (2010) The psychology of moral conviction. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 4:267–281. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00254.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Skitka LJ (2014) The psychological foundations of moral conviction. Advances in Experimental Moral Psychology 148

  • Skitka LJ, Bauman CW (2008) Moral conviction and political engagement. Polit Psychol 29:29–54. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00611.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Skitka LJ, Bauman CW, Sargis EG (2005) Moral conviction: another contributor to attitude strength or something more? J Pers Soc Psychol 88:895–917. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.895

    Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski JJ, Carlston DE (1987) Social judgment and social memory: the role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. J Pers Soc Psychol 52:689–699. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.4.689

    Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski JJ, Carlston DE (1989) Negativity and extremity biases in impression formation: a review of explanations. Psychol Bull 105:131–142. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.105.1.131

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern PC, Dietz T, Abel T, Guagnano GA, Kalof L (1999) A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: the case of environmental concern. Hum Ecol Rev 6:81–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoll-Kleemann S, O’Riordan T, Jaeger CC (2001) The psychology of denial concerning climate mitigation measures: evidence from Swiss focus group. Global Environ Chang 11:107–117. doi:10.1016/S0959-3780(00)00061-3

    Google Scholar 

  • Täuber S, Van Zomeren M (2013) Outrage towards whom? Threats to moral group status impede striving to improve via out-group-directed outrage. Eur J Soc Psychol 43:149–159. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1930

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE (2002) Social functionalist frameworks for judgment and choice: Intuitive politicians, theologians, and prosecutors. Psychol Rev 109:451–471. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.451

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock PE, Kristel OV, Elson SB, Green MC, Lerner JS (2000) The psychology of the unthinkable: taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. J Pers Soc Psychol 78:853–870. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.853

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas EF, McGarty C, Mavor KI (2009) Transforming “apathy into movement”: the role of prosocial emotions in motivating action for social change. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 13:310–333. doi:10.1177/1088868309343290

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, Haas IJ, Cunningham WA (2012) The importance of moral construal: moral versus non-moral construal elicits faster, more extreme, universal evaluations of the same actions. PLoS ONE 7:e48693. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048693

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Stekelenburg J, Klandermans B, Van Dijk WW (2009) Context matters: explaining how and why mobilizing context influences motivational dynamics. J Soc Issues 65:815–838. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01626.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren M (2013) Four core social-psychological motivations to undertake collective action. Soc Pers Psychol Compass 7:378–388. doi:10.1111/spc3.12031

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren M, Iyer A (2009) Introduction to the social and psychological dynamics of collective action. J Soc Issues 65:645–660. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01618.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren M, Postmes T, Spears R, Bettache K (2011) Can moral convictions motivate the advantaged to challenge social inequality? Extending the social identity model of collective action. Group Process Intergr Relat 14:735–753. doi:10.1177/1368430210395637

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren M, Postmes T, Spears R (2012) On conviction’s collective consequences: integrating moral conviction with the social identity model of collective action. Br J Soc Psychol 5:52–71. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2010.02000.x

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Zomeren M, Saguy T, Schellhaas FMH (2013) Believing in “making a difference” to collective efforts: participative efficacy beliefs as a unique predictor of collective action. Group Process Intergr Relat 16:618–634. doi:10.1177/1368430212467476

    Google Scholar 

  • Witte K, Allen M (2000) A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav 27:591–615. doi:10.1177/109019810002700506

    Google Scholar 

  • Wojciszke B (1994) Multiple meanings of behavior: construing actions in terms of competence or morality. J Pers Soc Psychol 67:222–232. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.222

    Google Scholar 

  • Wojciszke B (2005) Affective concomitants of information on morality and competence. Eur J Soc Psychol 10:60–70. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.10.1.60

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susanne Täuber.

Additional information

This article is part of a special issue on “Multidisciplinary perspectives on climate ethics” with guest editors Marco Grasso and Ezra M. Markowitz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Täuber, S., van Zomeren, M. & Kutlaca, M. Should the moral core of climate issues be emphasized or downplayed in public discourse? Three ways to successfully manage the double-edged sword of moral communication. Climatic Change 130, 453–464 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-014-1200-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-014-1200-6

Keywords

Navigation