Advertisement

Language Resources and Evaluation

, Volume 47, Issue 4, pp 1149–1161 | Cite as

The potentials and limitations of modelling concept concreteness in computational semantic lexicons with dictionary definitions

  • Oi Yee  Kwong
Original Paper
  • 224 Downloads

Abstract

This paper explores the feasibility of modelling concept concreteness perceived by humans and representing it in computational semantic lexicons, addressing an issue at the crossroads of computational linguistics, lexicography, and psycholinguistics. The inherent distinction between concrete words and abstract words in psychology has relied mostly on subjective human ratings. This practice is hardly scalable and does not consider the effect of polysemy. In view of this, we attempt to obtain a measure of concreteness from dictionary definitions comparable to human judgement, capitalising on conventional lexicographic assumptions and the regularities exhibited in the surface structures of sense definitions. The structural pattern of a definition is analysed and scored on a 7-point scale of concreteness ratings. The definition scores turned out to be quite effective for a dichotomous distinction between concrete and abstract concepts and more consistent with human ratings for the former. Beyond the two-way distinction, however, the results were more variable. The study has thus revealed the potentials and limitations of our approach, suggesting that different defining styles probably reflect the describability of concepts, and describability alone may not be sufficient for differentiating the degree of concreteness. The range of definition patterns has to be reconsidered, in combination with other inseparable factors constituting our perception of concreteness, for better modelling on a finer scale of concreteness distinction to enrich semantic lexicons for natural language processing.

Keywords

Concept concreteness Dictionary definitions Semantic lexicons Polysemy Computational lexicography 

Notes

Acknowledgments

The work described in this paper was partially supported by grants from the Research Grants Council of the Hong Kong Special Administrative Region, China (Project No. CityU 1508/06H), and the Department of Chinese, Translation and Linguistics of the City University of Hong Kong. The author would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments and suggestions.

References

  1. Amsler, R. (1981). A taxonomy for English nouns and verbs. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL’81), Stanford, pp. 133–138.Google Scholar
  2. Atkins, B. T. S., & Rundell, M. (2008). The oxford guide to practical lexicography. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bleasdale, F. A. (1987). Concreteness dependent associative priming: Separate lexical organization for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 582–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chodorow, M. S., Byrd, R. J., & Heidorn, G. E. (1985). Extracting semantic hierarchies from a large on-line dictionary. In Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL’85), Chicago, pp. 299–304.Google Scholar
  5. Fellbaum, C. (1998). WordNet: An electronic lexical database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Hanks, P. (1987). Definitions and explanations. In J. M. Sinclair (Ed.), Looking up: An account of the COBUILD project in lexical computing (Chapter 6). London, UK: HarperCollins Publishers.Google Scholar
  7. Johansson, R., & Nugues, P. (2008). Dependency-based syntactic-semantic analysis with PropBank and NomBank. In Proceedings of the 12th Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (CoNLL 2008), Manchester, pp. 183–187.Google Scholar
  8. Kroll, J. F., & Merves, J. S. (1986). Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 92–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Kwong, O.Y. (2012). Psycholinguistics, lexicography, and word sense disambiguation. In Proceedings of the 26th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information and Computation (PACLIC 26), Bali, Indonesia, pp. 408–417.Google Scholar
  11. Landau, S. I. (2001). Dictionaries: The art and craft of lexicography. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Monograph Supplement, 76(1, Pt.2), 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Rundell, M. (2006). More than one way to skin a cat: Why full-sentence definitions have not been universally adopted. In E. Corino, C. Marello, & C. Onesti (Eds.), Proceedings of the XII EURALEX International Congress (EURALEX 2006), Torino, Italy, pp. 323–337.Google Scholar
  15. Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1991). Why are abstract concepts hard to understand? In P. J. Schwanenflugel (Ed.), The psychology of word meanings (pp. 223–250). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
  16. Vossen, P., Meijs, W., & den Broeder, M. (1989). Meaning and structure in dictionary definitions. In B. Boguraev, & E. J. Briscoe (Eds.), Computational lexicography for natural language processing. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  17. Yore, L. D., & Ollila, L. O. (1985). Cognitive development, sex, and abstractness in grade one word recognition. Journal of Educational Research, 78, 242–247.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Chinese, Translation and LinguisticsCity University of Hong KongKowloonHong Kong

Personalised recommendations