Skip to main content
Log in

How domesticating fire facilitated the evolution of human cooperation

  • Published:
Biology & Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Controlled fire use by early humans could have facilitated the evolution of human cooperation. Individuals with regular access to the benefits of domestic fire would have been at an advantage over those with limited or no access. However, a campfire would have been relatively costly for an individual to maintain and open to free riders. By cooperating, individuals could have reduced maintenance costs, minimized free riding and lessened the risk of being without fire. Cooperators were more likely to survive and reproduce than uncooperative individuals because the former would have been better able to maximize a fire’s returns and enjoy regular access to its benefits. This is how the emergence of controlled fire use in Pleistocene human populations could have facilitated the evolution of cooperation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Interestingly, fire use may be the earliest human behaviour that involved a public good in a strict sense. Dubreuil (2010) has argued that cooperative breeding and hunting by Middle Pleistocene humans around 500 kya is indicative of increased cognitive control in this respect. While recognizing the significance of cooperative breeding, Vaesen (2011) counters that we have no evidence for this behaviour, and that cooperative hunting does not necessarily involve human kinds of cooperation. However, cooperative hunting in non-human animals does not involve transporting, cooking and sharing the food (Stiner et al. 2009).

References

  • Alperson-Afil N, Goren-Inbar N (2010) The Acheulian site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov volume II: ancient flames and controlled use of fire. Springer, Dordrecht

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley R (2001) The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsych Rev 11:1–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck I, Gärdenfors P (2003) Co-operation and communication in apes and humans. Mind Lang 18:484–501

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burton F (2009) Fire: the spark that ignited human evolution. University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark J, Harris J (1985) Fire and its role in early hominid lifeways. Afric Arch Rev 3:3–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Silva H, Hauert C, Traulsen A, Sigmund K (2010) Freedom, enforcement, and the social dilemma of strong altruism. J Evol Econ 20:203–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubreuil B (2010) Paleolithic public goods games: why human culture and cooperation did not evolve in one step. Biol Phil 25:53–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elton S (2008) The environmental context of human evolutionary history in Eurasia and Africa. J Anat 212:377–393

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fonseca-Azevedo K, Herculano-Houzel S (2012) Metabolic constraint imposes tradeoff between body size and number of brain neurons in human evolution. Pro Nat Acad Sci 109:18571–18576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C, Gowlett J, Dunbar R (2011) The social brain and the shape of the Palaeolithic. Camb Arch J 21:115–135

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan I (2010) The prehistoric development of clothing: archaeological implications of a thermal model. J Arch Method Theory 17:15–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goudsblom J (1992) The civilizing process and the domestication of fire. J World Hist 3:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett J (2006) The early settlement of northern Europe: fire history in the context of climate change and the social brain. Hum Palaeo and Prehist. C. R. Palevol 5:299–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett J (2010) Firing up the social brain. In: Dunbar R, Gamble C, Gowlett J (eds) Social brain, distributed mind (Proceedings of the British Academy: 158). Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gowlett J, Wrangham R (2013) Earliest fire in Africa: towards the convergence of archaeological evidence and the cooking hypothesis. Azania: Arch Res in Africa 48:5–30

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grove M, Pearce E, Dunbar R (2012) Fission-fusion and the evolution of hominin social systems. J Hum Evol 62:191–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauert C, Traulsen A, De Silva H, Nowak M, Sigmund K (2008) Public goods with punishment and abstaining in finite and infinite populations. Biol Theo 3:114–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (1993) Why hunter-gatherers work: an ancient version of the problem of public goods. Curr Anthropol 34:341–361

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry D (ed) (2003) Neanderthals in the Levant: behavioural organization and the beginnings of modern humanity. Continuum, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirata S (2009) Chimpanzee social intelligence: selfishness, altruism, and the mother-infant bond. Primates 50:3–11

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holloway R, Broadfield D, Yuan M (2004) The human fossil record: brain endocasts–the paleoneurological evidence, Volume 3. Wiley, New Jersey

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy S (2009) Mothers and others: the evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard Univesity Press, Cambridge MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenike M (2001) Nutritional ecology: diet, physical activity and body size. In: Panter-Brick C, Layton H, Rowley-Conwy P (eds) Hunter-gatherers: an interdisciplinary perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser I, Jensen K, Call J, Tomasello M (2012) Theft in an ultimatum game: chimpanzees and bonobos are insensitive to unfairness. Biol Lett. Published online 15th Aug, 1–6

  • Kurzban R, Houser D (2005) Experiments investigating cooperative types in humans: a complement to evolutionary theory and simulations. Pro Natl Acad Sci 102:1803–1807

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lalueza-Fox C, Rosas A, Estalrrich A, Gigli E, Campos P, Garcia-Tabernero A, Garcia-Vargas S, Sanchez-Quinto F, Ramirez O, Civit S, Bastir M, Huguet R, Santamaria D, Gilbert T, Willersiev E, de la Rasilla M (2011) Genetic evidence for patrilocal mating behavior among Neandertal groups. Pro Natl Acad Sci 108:250–253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lycett J, Norton C (2010) A demographic model for Palaeolithic technological evolution: the case of East Asia and the Movius Line. Quater Inter 211:55–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe F (2005) Hunter–Gatherers and human evolution. Evol Anthropol 14:54–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ofek H (2001) Second nature: economic origins of human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Parfitt S, Ashton N, Lewis S, Abel R, Coope G, Field M, Gale R, Hoare P, Larkin N, Lewis M, Karloukovski V, Maher B, Peglar S, Preece R, Whittaker J, Stringer C (2010) Early Pleistocene human occupation at the edge of the boreal zone in northwest Europe. Nature (July) 466:229–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor D, Williamson A, de Waal F, Brosnan S (2013) Chimpanzees play the ultimatum game. Pro Natl Acad Sci Early Edition

  • Roebroeks W, Villa P (2011) On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Pro Natl Acad Sci 108:5209–5214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolland N (2004) Was the emergence of home bases and domestic fire a punctuated event? A review of the Middle Pleistocene record in Eurasia. Asia Perspect 43:248–280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronen A (1998) Domestic fire as evidence for language. In: Takeru A, Aoki K, Bar-Yosef O (eds) Neanderthals and modern humans in Western Asia. Plenum Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoenemann T (2006) Evolution of the size and functional areas of the human brain. Annu Rev Anthropol 35:379–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shinada M, Yamagishi T (2007) Punishing free riders: direct and indirect promotion of cooperation. Evol Hum Behav 28:330–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stapert D, Johansen L (1999) Flint and pyrite: making fire in the stone age. Antiquity 73:765–777

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2007) Social intelligence, human intelligence and niche construction. Phil Trans Roy Soc B 362:719–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2012) Language, gesture, skill: the co-evolutionary functions of language. Phil Trans Roy Soc B 367:2141–2151

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiner M, Barkai C, Gopher A (2009) Cooperative hunting and meat sharing 400–200 kya at Qesem Cave, Israel. Pro Nat Acad Sci 106:13207–13212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2009) Origins of human communication. The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Twomey T (2011) Keeping fire: the cognitive implications of controlled fire use by Middle Pleistocene humans. Unpublished PhD Thesis, http://repository.unimelb.edu.au/10187/11103

  • Twomey T (2013) The cognitive implications of controlled fire use by Middle Pleistocene humans. Camb Arch J 23:113–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaesen K (2011) Cooperative feeding and breeding, and the evolution of cognitive control. Biol Phil 27:115–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Mark Vugt, Van Lange P (2006) The altruism puzzle: psychological adaptations for prosocial behaviour. In: Schaller M, Simpson J, Kendrick D (eds) Evolution and social psychology. Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • West SA, El Mouden C, Gardner A (2011) Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evol Hum Behav 32:231–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R (2009) Catching fire: how cooking made us human. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R, Carmody R (2009) The energetic significance of cooking. J Hum Evol 57:379–391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham R, Carmody R (2010) Human adaptation to the control of fire. Evol Anthropol 19:187–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Terrence Twomey.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Twomey, T. How domesticating fire facilitated the evolution of human cooperation. Biol Philos 29, 89–99 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9402-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9402-2

Keywords

Navigation