Skip to main content
Log in

Attenuating Physiological Arousal Through the Manipulation of Simple Hand Movements

  • Published:
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The current study tests whether manipulating simple motor movements can regulate one’s physiological reactivity to negative images. Healthy college age participants were randomly assigned to no tapping, steady tapping, or slow tapping conditions and viewed two sets of 15 negative images from the international affective picture system. Participants viewed the first image set without manipulation. During the second image set, they were instructed to tap at a steady pace, a slow pace or not at all. Steady tapping suppressed the vagal component of the cardiovascular defense response, and produced a significant increase in respiration rate and skin conductance level (SCL). Slow tapping suppressed the sympathetic and enhanced the vagal components of the cardiovascular defensive response, and produced a decrease in heart rate, SCL and skin conductance responses to negative images. Results suggest that manipulating simple motor movements is an effective way to both up-regulate and more importantly, down-regulate one’s physiological response to negative affective images. Manipulation of slow and simple motor movements may be an effective means to attenuate autonomic arousal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. IAPS images used: violence (6260, 6300, 6230, 6370, 6570.1, 6550, 6510, 9410, 6313, 6250.1) Mutilation (3068, 3053, 3060, 3010, 3064, 3170, 3130, 3063, 3069, 3080). Neutral (5500, 5510, 5520, 5530, 5531).

References

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bramble, D. M., & Carrier, D. R. (1983). Running and breathing in mammals. Science, 219, 251–256. doi:10.1126/science.6849136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body. Neuroscience, 3, 655–665. doi:10.1038/nrn894.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2003). A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 303–307. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00123-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craig, A. D. (2008). Interoception and emotion: A neuroanatomical perspective. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barret (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd edn., pp. 272–289). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuthbert, B. N., Bradleym, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1996). Probing picture perception: Activation and emotion. Psychophysiology, 33(2), 103–111. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02114.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Horowitz, M. E., Schwartz, G. E., & Goodman, D. M. (1981). Lateral differences in the latency between finger tapping and the heart beat. The Society for Psychophysiological Research, 18, 36–41. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01539.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, B. D., Galton, H. C., Morgan, R., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Meyer, M., Cusack, R., Lawrence, A. D., & Dalgleish, T. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21, 1835–1844. doi:10.1177/0956797610389191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández, M. C., & Vila, J. (1989). Sympathetic-parasympathetic mediation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Biological Psychology, 28(2), 123–133. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(89)90094-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Automatic self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 151–170). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder. Emotion (Washington, D. C.), 10, 83–91. doi:10.1037/a0018441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of personality and social psychology, 74(1), 224. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (1999). Emotion regulation: Past, present, future. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 551–573. doi:10.1080/026999399379186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of abnormal psychology, 106(1), 95–103. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A duel-process framework. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 400–412. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.544160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopp, H., Troy, A. S., & Mauss, I. B. (2011). The unconscious pursuit of emotion regulation: Implications for psychological health. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 532–545. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.532606.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, R. L., Carr, S. E., & MacLeod, D. A. (1990). Cardiac-locomotor coupling while finger tapping. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 1099–1104. doi:10.2466/PMS.71.8.1099-1104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohl, J., Koller, E. A., & Jager, M. (1981). Relation between pedalling and breathing rhythm. European Journal of Applied Physiological Occupational Physiology, 47, 223–237. doi:10.1007/BF00422468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koole, S. L., & Rothermund, K. (2011). “I feel better and don’t know why”: The psychology of implicit emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion, 25, 389–399. doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.550505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). Motivated attention: Affect, activation, and action. Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes (pp. 97–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assosciates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-7. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahler, D. A., Hunter, B., Lentine, T., & Ward, J. (1991). Locomotor-respiratory coupling develops in novice female rowers with training. Medical Science Sports Exercise, 23, 1362–1366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, I. B., Cook, L. C., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Automatic emotion regulation during anger provocation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 698–711. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, I. B., Evers, C., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006). How to bite your tongue without blowing your top: Implicit evaluation of emption regulation predicts affective responding to anger provocation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 589–602. doi:10.1177/0146167205283841.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, F., Vallbo, A. K., Olausson, H., Loken, L., & Wessberg, J. (2007). Discrimative touch and emotional touch. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 173–183. doi:10.1037/cjep2007019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mravec, O. K. (2010). Multilevel interactions between the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems: a minireview. Endocrine Regulations, 44, 69–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mund, M., & Mitte, K. (2012). The costs of repression: A meta-analysis on the relation between repressive coping and somatic diseases. Health psychology, 31(5), 640. doi:10.1037/a0026257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, R. P., Kamath, M. V., Floras, J. S., Stanley, J., Pang, C., Picton, P., & Young, Q. R. (2005). Heart rate variability biofeedback as a behavioral neurocardiac intervention to enhance vagal heart rate control. American Heart Journal, 149(6), 1137–1131. doi:10.1016/j.ahj.2005.03.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1975). Presidential address, 1975: The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction-as it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb00081.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polak, A. R., Witteveen, A. B., Denys, D., & Olff, M. (2015). Breathing Biofeedback as an Adjunct to Exposure in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Hastens the Reduction of PTSD Symptoms: A Pilot Study. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback, 40(1), 25–31. doi:10.1007/s10484-015-9268-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinsloo, G. E., Derman, W. E., Lambert, M. I., & Rauch, H. L. (2013). The effect of a single session of short duration biofeedback-induced deep breathing on measures of heart rate variability during laboratory-induced cognitive stress: A pilot study. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback, 38(2), 81–90. doi:10.1007/s10484-013-9210-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raio, C. M., Orederu, T. A., Palazzolo, L., Shurick, A. A., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). Cognitive emotion regulation fails the stress test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(37), 15139–15144. doi:10.1073/pnas.1305706110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramírez, I., Guerra, P., Perakakis, P., Anllo-Vento, L., & Vila, J. (2010). The dynamics of cardiac defense: From attention to action. Psychophysiology, 47(5), 879–887. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01008.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1391–1396. doi:10.1177/0956797611418350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallbo, A. B., & Wessberg, J. (1993). Organization of motor output in slow finger movements in man. Journal of Physiology, 469, 673–691. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019837.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vila, J., Pérez, M. N., Fernández, M. D. C., Pegalajar, J., & Sánchez, M. (1997). Attentional modulation of the cardiac defense response in humans. Psychophysiology, 34(4), 482–487. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02393.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wessberg, J., & Vallbo, A. B. (1995). Coding of pulsatile motor output by human muscle afferents during slow finger movements. Journal of Physiology, 485, 271–282. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020729.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilke, J. T., Lansing, R. W., & Rogers, C. A. (1975). Entrainment of respiration to repetitive finger tapping. Physiological Psychology, 3, 345–349. doi:10.3758/BF03326838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shaun S. Stearns.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stearns, S.S., Fleming, R. & Fero, L.J. Attenuating Physiological Arousal Through the Manipulation of Simple Hand Movements. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 42, 39–50 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-017-9350-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-017-9350-8

Keywords

Navigation