Skip to main content
Log in

Attempts to Suppress Episodic Memories Fail but do Produce Demand: Evidence from the P300-Based Complex Trial Protocol and an Implicit Memory Test

  • Published:
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Instructions to voluntarily suppress memories of a mock crime have been reported to result in decreased P300 amplitude during a P300-based concealed information test (CIT) and reduced autobiographical Implicit Association Test (aIAT) D scores, supporting successful suppression. However, one such study, (Hu et al., Psychological science 26(7):1098–1106, 2015) used the P300-based Complex Trial Protocol with a 50–50 target to nontarget ratio, which could impose much response switching and thereby drain cognitive resources, also resulting in reduced P300. The present study replicated Hu et al. (Psychological science 26(7):1098–1106, 2015) with one major variation—a less intrusive 20–80 target to nontarget ratio that required less response switching. Detection rates were high using both the brainwave-based CIT (90% accuracy) and the aIAT (87% accuracy). However we found no significant differences between the suppression and simple guilty groups on the major indices of concealed information detection, which compare probe and irrelevant P300 responses. While we did find that overall P300 amplitude was reduced in the suppression group, this reduction was not specific to probe responses. Additionally, while there were group differences in aIAT hit rates, there were no differences in aIAT D scores. Taken together, these findings suggest that the previously demonstrated reductions in P300 are a reflection of task demand rather than of effective voluntary memory suppression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Although the majority of Ps were female, previous research indicates negligible P300 gender differences in a visual oddball task (Rozenkrants and Polich 2008).

  2. The t-tests were one-tailed.

  3. One-tailed t-test.

  4. We acknowledge that Hu et al. (2015) used a different amplifier and slightly higher high-pass filter setting (0.3 Hz). However, the disparity in amplitude sizes seen here exceeds what might be explained by these differences (Duncan-Johnson and Donchin 1979).

  5. This $10 was in addition to participation compensation (either $10 or course credit). All Ps received the bonus but were initially told they would only receive it if found innocent.

References

  • Agosta, S., & Sartori, G. (2013). The autobiographical IAT: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 519.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Green, C. (2001). Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature, 410(6826), 366–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., & Levy, B. J. (2009). Suppressing unwanted memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 189–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. C., Ochsner, K. N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S. W., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303(5655), 232–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Shakhar, G. (2012). Current research and potential applications of the concealed information test: An overview. Frontiers in psychology, 3, 342.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bergström, Z. M., Anderson, M. C., Buda, M., Simons, J. S., & Richardson- Klavehn, A. (2013). Intentional retrieval suppression can conceal guilty knowledge in ERP memory detection tests. Biological Psychology, 94, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A. (Ed.), (2005). Intrusive thoughts in clinical disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd edn.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. H. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Science, 11, 357–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donchin, E., Kramer, A., & Wickens, C. (1986). Applications of brain event related potentials to problems in engineering psychology. In M. Coles, S. Porges & E. Donchin (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes and applications (pp. 702–710). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan-Johnson, C. C., & Donchin, E. (1979). The time constant in P300 recording. Psychophysiology, 16(1), 53–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Efron, B. (1979). Bootstrap methods: Another look at the jackknife. The Annals of Statistics, 7(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellwanger, J., Rosenfeld, J. P., Sweet, J. J., & Bhatt, M. (1996). Detecting simulated amnesia for autobiographical and recently learned information using the P300 event-related potential. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 23(1), 9–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farwell, L. A., & Donchin, E. (1991). The truth will out: Interrogative polygraphy (“lie detection”) with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 531–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, X., Bergström, Z. M., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2015). Suppressing unwanted autobiographical memories reduces their automatic influences evidence from electrophysiology and an implicit autobiographical memory test. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1098–1106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. (1993). On the neural generators of the P300 component of the event-related potential. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 90–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, A., Debener, S., Gratton, G., Junghöfer, M., Kappenman, E. S., Luck, S. J., Luu, P., Miller, G. A., & Yee, C. M. (2014). Committee report: Publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology, 51, 1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinberg, B., & Verschuere, B. (2016). The role of motivation to avoid detection in reaction time-based concealed information detection. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(1), 43–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, E. H., Selle, N. K., Elber, L., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2014). Memory detection with the Concealed Information Test: A meta analysis of skin conductance, respiration, heart rate, and P300 data. Psychophysiology, 51(9), 879–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meixner, J. B., Haynes, A., Winograd, M. R., Brown, J., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2009). Assigned versus random, countermeasure-like responses in the P300 based complex trial protocol for detection of deception: Task demand effects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 34(3), 209–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meixner, J. B., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). A mock terrorism application of the P300-based concealed information test. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 149–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meixner, J. B., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2014). Detecting knowledge of incidentally acquired, real-world memories using a P300-based concealed-information test. Psychological science, 25(11), 1994–2005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mertens, R., & Allen, J. J. (2008). The role of psychophysiology in forensic assessments: Deception detection, ERPs, and virtual reality mock crime scenarios. Psychophysiology, 45, 286–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2128–2148.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). P300 in detecting concealed information. Memory detection, 63–89.

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Shue, E., & Singer, E. (2007). Single versus multiple probe blocks of P300-based concealed information tests for autobiographical versus incidentally learned information. Biological Psychology, 74, 396–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Soskins, M., Bosh, G., & Ryan, A. (2004). Simple effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 41, 205–219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Winograd, M., Lui, A. M. ,Vandenboom, C., & Chedid, E. (2008). The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A new, countermeasure-resistant, accurate P300-based method for detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 45, 906–919

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Davydova, E., Ward, A., & Rosenfeld, L. (2017a). Financial incentive does not affect P300 to certain episodic and semantic probes in the P300-based Complex Trial Protocol (CTP) in malingering detection. Psychophysiology (in press).

  • Rosenfeld, J. P., Ward, A., Drapekin, J., Labkovsky, E., & Tullman, S. (2017b). Instructions to suppress semantic memory enhances or has no effect on P300 in a Concealed Information Test (CIT). International Journal of Psychophysiology (in press).

  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting thenull hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 225–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rozenkrants, B., & Polich, J. (2008). Affective ERP processing in a visual oddball task: arousal, valence, and gender. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(10), 2260–2265

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, G., Agosta, S., Zogmaister, C., Ferrara, S. D., & Castiello, U. (2008). How to accurately detect autobiographical events. Psychological Science, 19(8), 772–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P., & Presslich, O. (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 23(6), 695–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, W., Leuthold, H., & Matt, J. (1998). The expectancies that govern the P300 amplitude are mostly automatic and unconscious. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 149–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soskins, M., Rosenfeld, J. P., & Niendam, T. (2001). Peak-to-peak measurement of P300 recorded at 0.3 Hz high pass filter settings in intraindividual diagnosis: Complex vs. simple paradigms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40(2), 173–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception by manipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 141–142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1989). White bears and other unwanted thoughts: Suppression, obsession, and the psychology of mental control. London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thoughts suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Peter Rosenfeld.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors, both Ward and Rosenfeld, declare there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study, as required by the Northwestern University IRB which approved this study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ward, A.C., Rosenfeld, J.P. Attempts to Suppress Episodic Memories Fail but do Produce Demand: Evidence from the P300-Based Complex Trial Protocol and an Implicit Memory Test. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 42, 13–26 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-016-9348-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-016-9348-7

Keywords

Navigation