Introduction

Corvids, particularly magpies (Pica pica), have often been directly observed (Praus et al. 2014; Hanmer et al. 2017) or identified from physical evidence (Groom 1993; Chamberlain 1994) predating the eggs and chicks of UK songbirds in the nest. However, investigation at the regional and national population level has found little evidence of a causal link between increases in corvid populations and declines in songbird populations (Thomson et al. 1998; Newson et al. 2010). Meta-analyses of predator removal studies have not found a uniformly positive effect of corvid removal on songbird productivity or abundance (Holt et al. 2008; Madden et al. 2015). Nevertheless, some localised experimental studies have found increases in nest survival (White et al. 2014; Sage and Aebischer 2017), and subsequent growth in some songbird breeding populations (Stoate and Szczur 2006; Fletcher et al. 2010), following corvid removal. There are several possible explanations for this confused picture.

Firstly, corvids’ conspicuous behaviour and diurnal activity may make them appear disproportionately responsible for nest predation, compared to more inconspicuous nocturnal predators such as rodents (Birkhead 1991). Corvids may simply not be common nest predators. Secondly, the eggs and chicks predated by corvids may be part of the population that would otherwise be lost due to different causes (compensatory mortality) (Errington 1946). In this case, increased corvid nest predation would not necessarily limit songbird population numbers. Thirdly, the breeding biology of some songbird species may render their nests more or less likely to be predated by corvids (Martin 1993) and although increased corvid numbers might affect susceptible species, this effect may be masked when studies consider population trends of multiple species. Finally, it may be that some individual corvids predate a disproportionate number of nests and if these particularly predatory individuals are not removed, songbird breeding success or population numbers do not increase.

Several factors may cause corvids to differ in how likely they are to predate songbird nests including variation in their external environment. For example, if the habitat that predators utilise (their home range) provides less concealment for nests, perhaps because hedges are severely cut and thin, predators may be more likely to detect and predate nests (Dunn et al. 2016).

Predators may also differ in their predation behaviour due to variation in internal factors. Predators which differ in their age, sex or breeding status may take different prey types (Dickman 1988; Sacks et al. 1999; Odden et al. 2002). Corvids such as magpies and crows (Corvus corone) can differ in breeding status. Their populations comprise of territorial breeding individuals as well as non-breeding itinerant individuals (Cramp and Perrins 1994). The breeding individuals defend a territory around their nest from intrusions by their predators and conspecifics during the breeding season. They also generally limit their foraging to within these territories (Birkhead 1991), possibly to facilitate this territorial defence (Martindale 1982). This restricted foraging area, and increased local habitat knowledge it facilitates, may cause territorial breeding corvids to encounter, and predate, more nests than non-breeding individuals which forage over a wider area (Møller 1988; Sálek 2004).

In addition to causing spatial variation in nest predation risk, these environmental and internal factors could also cause nest predation risk to vary temporally. The habitat is likely to change through the year. For instance, as vegetation grows, the protection from predation it provides may increase, dependent on the mix of plant species. Variation in internal factors, such as breeding status, may have a temporal effect on predators’ likelihood to take particular prey. Breeding individuals may switch their prey preference in response to the nutritional needs of their young (Annett and Pierotti 1989). For example, predation of nests by corvids might be higher during particular stages of the corvids’ own breeding season (Suvorov et al. 2012).

Alternatively, or in addition to these external and internal factors, predators may differ at an individual level in their propensity to predate nests (Woo et al. 2008). Although, at a population level corvids appear to have a generalist diet (Holyoak 1968), this could mask individual specialisation on different prey (Dickman and Newsome 2015). Some corvids may specialise on songbird eggs or chicks. Individuals may differ in their predation behaviour for a variety of reasons. If prey requires specific handling strategies and predators are limited in the number of strategies that they can learn, individuals may focus on different prey (Woo et al. 2008). Individuals may learn specific foraging preferences from their parents or others in their social group (Slagsvold and Wiebe 2011). Additionally, differences in personality have been shown to affect foraging preferences, for example bolder individuals may be more likely to dominate particular foraging habitats (Patrick and Weimerskirch 2014).

Farmland songbirds are a group of species in long-term population decline (DEFRA 2016). Identifying specific factors which cause corvids to differ in their predation of farmland songbird nests may demonstrate where predator and/or habitat management could be focused to achieve maximum increases in songbird productivity. More targeted conservation management is increasingly favoured due to economic and, in the case of predator removal, ethical concerns. If particular individuals or guilds of individuals disproportionately predate species of interest, independent of variation in the external environment, it may be possible to identify and selectively remove these ‘problem individuals’ (Swan et al. 2017). Alternatively, if external factors such as habitat variation drive differences in predation rates, prioritising the creation of habitat that is associated with reduced predation rates could be particularly beneficial.

We aimed to identify factors which caused variation in nest predation rates in a farmland environment using artificial nests. Specifically, we explored whether corvids were disproportionately responsible for predation, by monitoring the identity of nest predators using trail cameras and wax-filled eggs, which retain the beak or gnaw marks of the predators attempting to eat them (Bodey et al. 2009; Ludwig et al. 2012). We then explored whether spatial and temporal patterns of variation in predation could be explained by factors pertaining to the behaviour of local corvid predators. By presenting sets of artificial nests at different locations repeatedly throughout the breeding season, we could assess whether predation: (i) varied depending on predator breeding status, in this case whether predation was higher inside the territories of breeding magpies; (ii) varied temporally over the breeding season; (iii) varied within or between locations, specifically did predation differ between individual magpie territories; and (iv) varied due to habitat variation, measured as difference in hedge structure.

Methods

Field site

The study was carried out over 15 km2 of farmland in Warwickshire, England. This lowland area was dominated by farmland, with small fragments of deciduous and plantation woodland. The farmland was both arable and pastoral. The arable crops were mainly cereals, such as wheat (Triticum aestivum) and barley (Hordeum vulgare), and break crops, such as oil seed rape (Brassica napus) and field peas (Pisum sativum). The majority of the pastoral land was improved grassland grazed by sheep and cattle. Field size was relatively small (3.11 ± 0.28 ha) and most fields were bordered by either managed (mechanically cut) or unmanaged hedgerows.

Measurement of nest predation

We constructed artificial nests from chicken wire lined and woven with hay and grass. These nests mimicked blackbird (Turdus merula) nests found on the field site in size, shape, and use of hay and grass. Nests were baited with one real quail’s egg and one wax-filled quail’s egg.

We presented artificial nests (n = 460) repeatedly at twenty-four selected hedgerow locations between 24 March 2016 and 26 July 2016. At each location, five artificial nests, each separated by ~ 10 m, were placed in realistic nest positions on a transect along the hedgerow. Each nest was exposed for 5 days and, to avoid alerting predators, nests were not visited during this time. Nests were considered predated if either egg was missing or damaged within the nest. Two methods of nest predator identification were used. Impressions left on the wax-filled eggs allowed us to broadly classify predators (categories used were medium-sized birds, small birds, small mammals and unknown). A subset of nests, approximately one third (n = 151) were also monitored using trail cameras (Bushnell Trophy Camera HD) to allow more detailed identification of predators. Cameras were programmed to take 3 photographs (5 megapixel resolution) per trigger with a 1-s trigger interval. The sensitivity of the passive infra-red sensor was set to “high” to maximise motion sensitivity. Predators were identified as animals seen at the nest interacting with nest contents. Cameras were positioned within the vegetation of the hedgerows to prevent them providing a visual indication of nest location. A randomly selected subset (n = 30) of the nests deployed were old, natural thrush nests, collected from the field site over winter, as oppose to wire nests constructed by us. This meant any effect of our use of man-made materials, as oppose to nesting materials used by birds, could be assessed.

Assessing the effect of predator breeding status on nest predation

Twelve (of twenty-four) locations where artificial nest transects were presented were situated within active magpie territories (magpie-present), and twelve were positioned at randomly selected sites where breeding magpies were absent (magpie-absent).

We identified the active magpie territories using a combination of methods. First, old magpie nest sites were located over the winter and early spring (before bud burst). This provided a likely territory centre, as nest sites were frequently reused (Birkhead 1991; LC personal observation). Second, we established that the territory was in use by locating the active nest site. Nest site use was confirmed by observing and recording locations of magpies in and around the nest site during the breeding season.

Active magpie nests in the best characterised area of the field site were found less than 200 m apart. Sites over 200 m from known magpie nest sites (even those believed to be inactive) were therefore defined as outside of magpie territories (magpie-absent). Areas which could not be visually searched for magpie nests (blocks of woodland over approx. 0.5 ha, and urban areas including gardens) were excluded from this experiment.

The transects in magpie-present locations were centred on the active magpie nest within the magpie territory; the first artificial nest of the transect was placed ~ 10 m from the active magpie nest if the nest was in a hedgerow, or at the closest point on the nearest hedgerow (< 25 m away) if the magpie nest was in a tree/copse. The transects in magpie absent locations were centred on randomly selected sites that matched magpie nest sites; the first artificial nest was placed at a random point on the hedgerow.

Assessing the effect of temporal variation on nest predation

Artificial nest transects were presented at the 24 locations in six blocks of four locations (two in magpie-present, two in magpie-absent locations). Nest transects were presented at each block, for 5 days each time, in April, May, June and July 2016. Nest transects were therefore presented at each block four times, except one block which was repeated three times due to logistical constraints, this meant 460 nests were presented in total.

To assess the relationship between temporal variation in nest predation rates and predator breeding phenology, magpie breeding was concurrently monitored on this field site (observations of active nests were made approximately every 4 days). Magpie young were found to fledge the nest between late-May and mid-June. This is comparable to regional estimates based on the BTO’s Nest Record Scheme (Joys and Crick 2004).

Assessing the effect of variation between individual predators on nest predation

We aimed to identify individual variation in magpie predation behaviour by examining differences in recorded predation rates at each location. As members of the local magpie population were trapped and marked with colour rings we could identify individual territory owners; marked birds that were repeatedly observed or re-trapped near specific active nests were assumed to be the territory owners (Diaz-Ruiz et al. 2010). By comparing the identity of the territory owners to the identity of colour ringed individuals observed predating nests within those territories (on trail camera photos) we could see if differences in predation rates between locations related to differences in predation by individual territorial magpies.

Assessing the effect of habitat variation on nest predation

Hedgerows at each nest location were crudely categorised as either open (above 2 m, with an open structure, generally unmanaged and composed of a mix of tree/shrub species) or closed (below 2 m in height, narrow and dense and composed of hawthorn (Crataegus spp.) and blackthorn (Prunus spinosa)).

Statistical analysis

Firstly, we assessed the effect of aspects of the experimental design on predation rate (number of nests predated/ total number of nests). This was captured by three measures: the presence of cameras; the type of nest (man-made or made by birds); and the position of the nests along a transect (proximity to magpie nest). We assessed the effect of these measures on nest predation using chi-squared tests and found that predation did not significantly differ between nests with and without cameras (χ21, n = 460 = 2.54, p = 0.11), between man-made nests or nests made by birds (χ21, n = 460 = 0.94, p = 0.33), or with nest position within a transect (χ21, n = 460 = 0.91, p = 0.92).

We then used a generalised linear mixed modelling (GLMM) approach to assess the effect of the factors of interest (magpie presence, time in breeding season and habitat variation) on artificial nest transect predation. Inclusion of additional factors in these models led to model over-parametrisation and reduced model fit (Grueber et al. 2011; Thomas et al. 2015). Therefore, the factors related to experimental design (camera presence, man-made/made by birds, nest position within a transect), which were shown to have no effect on artificial nest predation, were excluded from these analyses.

We fitted proportion of nest transect predated (number of nests predated vs. number of nests not predated) as a binomial response in a GLMM with binomial error distribution and a logit-link function. Magpie territory presence (magpie-present vs. magpie-absent), time of nest transect presentation (date converted to a continuous numeric variable where day 1 corresponds to the first day artificial nests were presented, which was 24 March 2016), and hedge type (open/closed) were used as explanatory variables. To account for changes in habitat and magpie activity throughout the breeding season, the interaction terms date × magpie presence and date × hedge type were included. Artificial nest location was fitted as a random effect. We included location as a random effect to account for potential spatial autocorrelation between the repeated presentations of transects of nests at the same location and to explicitly examine differences in predation rates between locations, not accounted for by the fixed effects.

To examine if factors predicting predation rates differed depending on predator identity we initially fitted two maximal models; the first included all instances of predation (A) and the second included only predation attributed to medium-sized bird predators (B), effectively magpies (see “Results”).

The significance of explanatory terms in both models was evaluated by removing variables one at a time and using chi-squared tests to assess the change in model deviance (Crawley 2005). The first-order interaction terms were removed from models before the significance of fixed effects included in them was assessed (Crawley 2012; Fox et al. 2013). Non-significant interactions were excluded from minimal models, but all fixed effects were retained. The random effect (nest location) was tested using a likelihood ratio test of the maximal model, with and without the random effect (Westneat et al. 2014). Model assumptions were assessed using diagnostic plots (Thomas et al. 2015).

All analyses were carried out in R ver. 3.1.2 (R Core Team 2017). GLMMs were constructed using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2014).

Results

Predator identity

Across all presentations of all artificial nest transects, 133 of the total 460 nests (28.9%) were predated. Three guilds of nest predator were identified using either impressions left on wax eggs or trail camera photos or a combination of the two. The first of these, small birds (including songbirds), are not commonly known to be nest predators, but appeared to interact with artificial nest contents in a very small number of cases. The second guild of predators, small mammals, were identified as predators in 23.5% of cases where predators could be identified (n = 81) (Fig. 1). In cases where trail camera photos were available (n = 6), these small mammals were observed to be rodents, such as brown rats (Rattus norvegicus). No other mammals were observed predating nests on camera and although larger mammals, such as red foxes (Vulpes vulpes), were present on the field site, there was no evidence they predated the artificial nests. The third guild of nest predators, medium-sized birds, were by far the most frequently identified guild of nest predators (identified in 70.3% of cases where identification was possible (n = 81)) (Fig. 1). Corvids were the only medium-sized birds observed predating nests on camera (48.5% of the 33 predated nests monitored by cameras). Of these corvids, 87.5% were magpies. There was 1 instance of jackdaw predation (Corvus monedula) and 1 of jay (Garrulus glandarius). We therefore assume that magpies were the major predators of our artificial nests. In cases where the wax-filled egg was removed from artificial nest or untouched, and there were no camera observations, we could not determine predator identity (Table 1). Camera observations suggested that neither mammals nor birds were more likely to leave eggs untouched or to remove them.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Nest outcome for all predated artificial nests (n = 133)

Table 1 Count of the fate of predated artificial nests in magpie-absent (n = 60) and magpie-present (n = 73) transects (expressed as percentage of total predated nests in parenthesis)

Factors affecting artificial nest predation

When predation by all predators was considered (model A) magpie presence did not influence predation rate. Time in the breeding season was significant, with predation rates generally declining over the breeding season (Table 2A). However, when we examined factors that predicted only predation by medium-sized birds, the results changed (model B). The effect of magpie presence on the proportion of nests predated by medium-sized birds varied temporally. Although overall approximately 75% more nests were predated in magpie-present locations, this disparity in predation rates between magpie-present and magpie-absent transects was only evident later in the breeding season. For example, in the last presentation of artificial nests (in early July), 20% of nests were predated by medium-sized birds in magpie-present locations, whereas none were predated in magpie-absent locations (Table 2B, Fig. 2).

Table 2 Models showing the factors causing variation in the rate of artificial nest predation by (A) all predators and (B) medium-sized birds. Non-significant interaction terms are not shown
Fig. 2
figure 2

Percentage of nests predated by medium-sized birds in magpie-present (white bars) and magpie-absent (black bars) locations for each month

In both models (A and B), the specific location of nest transects, which was fitted as a random effect, was also highly significant (Table 2). Accounting for the effect of the fixed factors (magpie presence, hedge type and time in the breeding season), predation rates, by all predators and by medium-sized birds only, differed between locations. For example, at some locations over 70% of artificial nests presented were predated whereas at other locations less than 10% of nests were predated (Fig. 3). We examined the identity of individual magpies observed on camera predating at specific locations, and these observations indicated that higher predation at specific locations was driven by increased predation by local magpies. The individual colour-ringed magpies that were identified predating nest transects were only observed predating magpie-present transects located within their own territories, or in magpie-absent areas immediately adjacent to their territories. On the occasions when unringed birds were seen predating nests, the transects were in territories where the territory holders were not ringed.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Average percentage of nest transects predated per presentation ± 1 SE at magpie-present (white bars) and magpie-absent (black bars) sites

Discussion

Almost 30% of artificial nests placed in hedgerows in UK farmland were predated and magpies were the most frequently identified predators. Several factors were found to affect the predation of the artificial nests, but the effect of these different factors was, at least partially, dependent on the identity of the nest predators. The probability of a nest being predated, by any predator, decreased over the course of the breeding season, as hedgerow vegetation became denser. However, when only nest predation by medium-sized birds (most likely magpies) was considered, there was a greater probability that nests placed in close proximity to active magpie nests were predated late in the breeding season (when magpies were provisioning fledglings). We also found evidence that nests placed in some territories were more likely to be predated regardless of environmental or temporal factors. Consequently, we suggest that the effect corvid predators have on songbird nests may vary due to differences in breeding status, time in the breeding season, and possibly due to additional differences in foraging behaviour or motivation between territory holders.

Predation of artificial nests

When predators of our artificial nests could be identified (using wax-filled eggs and/or trail camera photos), 70% of predation was attributed to medium-sized birds. The medium-sized bird nest predators present on the field site were limited to corvids (largely jackdaws, jays and magpies) and previous work suggests that magpies are the most likely of these to forage in farmland hedgerows (Angelstam 1986; Cramp et al. 1994). This was supported by our observations; 87% of the medium-sized birds identified on camera predating nests were magpies. Therefore, throughout this discussion we presume the majority of this medium-sized bird predation was due to magpies. One explanation for the disproportionately high predation by magpies we observed is that one of our methods of predator identification (using marks in wax-filled eggs) might lead to an underestimate of mammalian predation. Wax-filled eggs have an unnatural smell and may have been less attractive to mammals, which rely on olfactory cues to find prey, than to birds which tend to utilise visual cues (Rangen et al. 2000). However, it did not appear that mammalian predators were taking real eggs and leaving wax-filled eggs untouched. Marks in our wax-filled eggs revealed that they were attacked by various mammalian predators and in the minority of predated nests where wax-filled eggs were untouched we rarely found evidence which suggested that the real eggs had been taken by mammals, such as shell fragments left in the nest (Best 1978; Marini and Melo 1998).

We are therefore confident that we have correctly classified the predators of our artificial nests. However, we could not directly assess whether the spatial and temporal patterns of predation observed, and the nest predators identified, were representative of natural nests in the same environment (Wilson et al. 1998; Pärt and Wretenberg 2002). For example, in the case of the latter, it has been suggested that small birds do not predate natural nests as frequently as they predate artificial nests as they are often deterred from natural nests by the breeding pair (Zanette 2002). It is also possible that the presence of researchers around artificial nest sites attracted particular predator species (Major 1990; Major and Kendal 1996), especially visually orientated avian predators (Westmoreland and Best 1985; Götmark 1992). Corvids particularly can learn to associate individual humans with specific actions (Lee et al. 2011); in this study, some magpies may have formed an association between the researcher and the presence of artificial nests (Götmark et al. 1990; Strang 1980). Conversely, other research has shown that human visitation of nests does not influence predation rates (Fletcher et al. 2005; Lambert and Kleindorfer 2006), and even that corvids can be deterred by human observers (Götmark and Ahlund 1984). In this particular case, any association between the researcher and artificial nests may have been weakened as the researcher was regularly present on the field site and interacting with the habitat for reasons unrelated to the positioning of artificial nests.

Natural songbird nests were monitored on the field site concurrent to this study, but the sample size was relatively small, and we could not identify the specific nest predators responsible as using the remains of natural nests to identify nest predators has been shown to be unreliable (Lariviere 1999; Mallord et al. 2012). Nonetheless, the natural songbird nests monitored were also found to suffer high rates of predation and the high proportion of avian predation suffered by our artificial nests is comparable to that observed in studies of natural nests in similar environments (Chamberlain 1994; Hatchwell et al. 1996). Artificial nests can experience patterns of nest predation comparable with coexisting natural nests, particularly when an attempt is made to mimic natural eggs and nests (Roos 2002; Pehlak and Lohmus 2008). The nests we constructed were modelled on thrush nests found on the study site and did not differ in predation rates compared to old thrush nests when both were baited with quail’s eggs. We therefore suggest that the predation of our artificial nests can provide insight into variation in nest predation and nest predator behaviour if not a direct representation of natural nest predation.

Effect of magpie presence on nest predation

Nest predation by all predators varied temporally; the rate of predation attributed to all predators declined towards the end of the breeding season, probably because nests became increasingly concealed due to growth in hedgerow vegetation (Söderström 2001; Rodewald et al. 2009). Predation by magpies on nests placed further from active magpie nests (outside of magpie territories) also showed the same trend, suggesting predation by non-territorial magpies (identified on the field site as part of an ongoing trapping program) may also have been reduced when foliage was denser.

However, predation specifically by magpies within magpie territories did not decline over the course of the breeding season despite equivalent vegetation growth. Territorial magpies have a more restricted foraging range than non-territorial birds (Vines 1981). They may therefore more thoroughly and frequently explore their foraging range (Møller 1988) and thus continue to encounter songbird nests within it even when nests are harder to find. Factors other than variation in habitat use may also have caused nests in areas used by breeding magpies to be relatively more vulnerable late in the breeding season. Magpies provision their young within their territory for at least 4 weeks after fledging (Birkhead 1991); therefore (considering magpie fledging dates on this site), it is likely magpies were feeding dependent fledglings during this period. When provisioning their young magpies, like other bird species (Sasvari and Hegyi 1998; Davoren and Burger 1999), may preferentially seek out protein-rich prey such as eggs which could benefit the developing fledglings (Annett and Pierotti 1989).

Nonetheless, this apparent difference in magpie predation between magpie-present and magpie-absent sites did not affect overall survival of artificial nests. Although predation was still elevated at magpie-present sites, when predation by predators other than magpies was included in the analysis, the difference was not significant. This might suggest that other predators compensated, at least partially, for any differences in magpie nest predation (Ellis-Felege et al. 2012).

Effect of location on artificial nest predation

Although nests placed in spatial association with an active magpie nest did suffer increased nest predation rates by magpies, not all nest transects placed in magpie territories suffered high predation. When considering either all predation or only predation by magpies, there was variation between locations, independent of magpie nest presence. Some artificial nest transects directly under active magpie nests were not heavily predated. Conversely, some transects identified as having higher predation rates were not adjacent to magpie nests. The latter case may indicate that non-territorial magpies were significant nest predators at some locations throughout the breeding season. However, non-territorial magpies are ephemeral in any particular area (Birkhead 1991) and are unlikely to be responsible for the consistently high nest predation observed at specific locations over four separate presentations of the artificial nests. Breeding territorial corvids may forage further from their nests to exploit particular, sparse, resources (Neatherlin and Marzluff 2004), and it seems more likely that the magpie-absent transects which had higher predation rates were within the foraging range of a nearby breeding magpie pair. The predatory individuals that could be identified on camera were all part of the neighbouring breeding pair adjacent to the magpie-absent transect.

However, this does not explain why we did not observe high predation rates on some magpie-present transects. Some territorial magpies did not repeatedly predate songbird nests around their own nest, even late in the breeding season when magpie predation of nests in magpie-present transects was highest. Individual predators within a population have been shown to differ in their foraging behaviour, both in prey choice and foraging location (Graham et al. 2011; Patrick and Weimerskirch 2014). Territorial magpies may have differed, at an individual level, in the extent to which they engaged in nest predation. Our experimental design, in which nests were repeatedly placed in the same location, may have emphasised any differences between territory holders. Corvids, including magpies, are able to remember and re-visit specific feeding locations (Sonerud and Fjeld 1987; Birkhead 1991; Zinkivskay et al. 2009). It may have been that individuals which encountered and predated nests early in breeding season may have developed a search image for our artificial nests and this experience could have facilitated relatively increased rates of nest predation later in the breeding season (Møller 1988; Vigallon et al. 2005). Further research would be required to understand if and why particular magpies are more likely to predate eggs and to provide a mechanism by which these individuals could be characterised.

Critically, as the data included in our analysis were collected in only 1 year and at one site, we cannot be certain that the higher predation rates observed in certain locations were because particularly predatory individuals held those territories, rather than because of some inherent property of those territories (for example the presence of a specific habitat characteristic) that we did not consider. Alternatively, perhaps there was some interaction between the properties of a territory and the behaviour of the territory holders such that the owners of particular territories were more likely to predate nests within them. For example, the owners of the best quality territories may be dominant older birds (Birkhead 1991) which also be more experienced predators.

Further fine-scale multi-year analysis relating magpie territory ownership to nest predation rates within those territories would be necessary to disentangle the relative influence of habitat variation between territories and magpie identity. In any case, identifying either territory holders or territories which are associated with an increased risk of songbird nest predation may indicate where management of predators and/or habitat could be targeted (Swan et al. 2017).

Implications of findings

Further work relating the patterns of artificial nest predation we found to the survival of natural nests would provide additional support for the application of our findings to management (Burke et al. 2004). Nonetheless, considering the effect of the factors found to explain variation in the likelihood of artificial nests being predated in this study could facilitate the identification of natural nests most susceptible to predation. Targeting management specifically towards improving the survival of these vulnerable nests may be more likely to increase the overall productivity of threatened songbird populations.

Targeted management could include improving nesting habitat in areas where predators are likely to take nests. For example, by increasing the protection provided by nest vegetation near to magpie nests (where nests were relatively more vulnerable late in the breeding season) (Dunn et al. 2016). Additionally, if we can identify the states or personality of individuals especially likely to predate songbird nests and directly target these individuals for removal or behavioural change (e.g. through diversionary feeding (Redpath et al. 2001)), then we might expect there to be greater benefits from control and management strategies (Blackwell et al. 2016; Swan et al. 2017). However, the consequences of removing particularly predatory individuals should be carefully considered. In our study, we could not distinguish whether the elevated predation risk was associated with territory holders, their specific territories or a combination of the two. It may be that removal of magpies from territories where predation rates are high would lead to other magpies replacing them and engaging in equally high rates of nest predation.

Furthermore, although magpies were identified as major nest predators in this study, the role of other predator species should also be considered. We found evidence that other predators compensated, at least partially, for differences in the rates magpie nest predation. Any management strategy aimed at improving bird breeding success should take into account the effect of the predator community as a whole.