Skip to main content
Log in

Automatic place-value activation in magnitude-irrelevant parity judgement

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research on multi-digit number processing suggests that, in Arabic numerals, their place-value magnitude is automatically activated, whenever a magnitude-relevant task was employed. However, so far, it is unknown, whether place-value is also activated when the target task is magnitude-irrelevant. The current study examines this question using the parity congruency effect in two-digit numbers: It describes that responding to decade-digit parity congruent numbers (e.g., 35, 46; same parity of decades and units) is faster than to decade-digit parity incongruent numbers (e.g., 25; 36; different parities of decades and units). Here we investigate the (a-) symmetry of the parity congruency effect; i.e. whether it makes a difference whether participants are assessing the parity of the unit digit or the decade digit. We elaborate, how and why such an asymmetry is related to place-value processing, because the parity of the unit digit only interferes with the parity of the decade digit, while the parity of the decade digit interferes with both the parity of the unit digit and the integrated parity of the whole two-digit number. We observed a significantly larger parity congruency effect in the decade parity decision than in the unit parity decision. This suggests that automatic place-value processing also takes place in a typical parity judgment task, in which magnitude is irrelevant. Finally, because of the cross-lingual design of the study, we can show that these results and their implications were language-independent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Place-value activation could in principle be further distinguished in approximate and exact place-value activation. Approximate place-value activation would refer to the approximate value of number magnitude on a fuzzy mental number line (Dehaene, 2001; Dehaene, Dupoux, and Mehler, 1990). A person (e.g., a child in a number line estimation task) would know that 92 is somehow larger than 29, which is comprised of the same digits, but on different positions, but may not be able to locate it exactly on a number line. For exact place-value activation, the exact value as indexed by the Arabic number system is derived. For the number 72, one would know that it is an even number, a multiplication table result of 8*9, and not just some approximate magnitude between 70 and 80. The parity derived from such an exact integration of place-value activation of both digits (i.e., 90 and 2 for 92) is termed “integrated parity” or sometimes “place-value integration” in this manuscript.

  2. This setup allows examining the SNARC effect (Dehaene et al., 1993) as well. This was not the main objective of this experiment, but we report the results in Appendix 1.

  3. Discrepancy between RT and accuracy data in numerical cognition have already been reported (see eg., Ratcliff & McKoon, 2018; Ratcliff, Thompson, & McKoon, 2015). Such differential effects can be accounted for in future research by using the diffusion models.

  4. We would like to thank Attila Krajcsi for pointing out this alternative explanation.

References

  • Bahnmueller, J., Maier, C. A., Göbel, S. M., & Moeller, K. (2019). Direct evidence for linguistic influences in two-digit number processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(6), 1142–1150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bahnmueller, J., Moeller, K., Mann, A., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2015). On the limits of language influences on numerical cognition–no inversion effects in three-digit number magnitude processing in adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1216.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M. (2019). How many participants do we have to include in properly powered experiments? A tutorial of power analysis with reference tables. Journal of Cognition, 2(3), 16.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cipolotti, L., & Butterworth, B. (1995). Toward a multiroute model of number processing: Impaired number transcoding with preserved calculation skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(4), 375–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2013). Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(10), 1974–1991.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., Schroeder, P. A., Soltanlou, M., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2018). More space, better mathematics: Is space a powerful tool or a cornerstone for understanding arithmetic? In K. S. Mix & M. T. Battista (Eds.), Visualizing mathematics: The role of spatial reasoning in mathematical thought (pp. 77–116). Cham: Springer International Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., Soltanlou, M., Reips, U.-D., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2019a). The SNARC and MARC effects measured online: Large-scale assessment methods in flexible cognitive effects. Behavior Research Methods, 51(4), 1676–1692.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., van Dijck, J.-P., Georges, C., Masson, N., Goebel, S., Willmes, K., M. Pesenti, C. Schiltz, Nuerk, H.-C. (2019). A Minority pulls the sample mean: on the individual prevalence of robust group-level cognitive phenomena–the instance of the SNARC effect, Preprint on https://psyarxiv.com/bwyr3/

  • Dehaene, S. (2001). Précis of the number sense. Mind and Language, 16(1), 16–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626–641.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dowker, A., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2016). Linguistic influences on mathematics. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1035.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition—From single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1461–1483.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganor-Stern, D., & Tzelgov, J. (2008). Across-notation automatic numerical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 430–437.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganor-Stern, D., Tzelgov, J., & Ellenbogen, R. (2007). Automaticity and two-digit numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 483–496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • García-Orza, J., Estudillo, A. J., Calleja, M., & Rodríguez, J. M. (2017). Is place-value processing in four-digit numbers fully automatic? Yes, but not always. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24(6), 1906–1914.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Göbel, S. M., Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2014). Language affects symbolic arithmetic in children: the case of number word inversion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 17–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, T., Scheuringer, A., & Pletzer, B. (2018). Sex differences and functional hemispheric asymmetries during number comparison. Biology of Sex Differences, 9(1), 3.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory and Cognition, 10(4), 389–395.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heubner, L., Cipora, K., Soltanlou, M., Schlenker, M.-L., Lipowska, K., Goebel, S. M., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2018). A mental odd-even continuum account: Some numbers may be “more odd” than others, and some numbers may be “more even” than others. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1081.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, T. M. (1990). An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits. Memory and Cognition, 18(1), 40–46.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hohol, M., Cipora, K., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2017). Bringing back the balance: domain-general processes are also important in numerical cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 499.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (2003). Spatial asymmetries in the flanker-congruency effect: Attentional scanning is biased by flanker orientation. Psychology Science, 45(1), 63–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopko, D. R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R. L., & Hunt, M. K. (2003). The abbreviated math anxiety scale (AMAS) construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment, 10(2), 178–182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S., Klein, E., Graf, M., Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., & Willmes, K. (2015). Embodied markedness of parity? Examining handedness effects on parity judgments. Psychological Research, 79(6), 963–977.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S., Klein, E., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Moeller, K. (2014). Decimal fraction representations are not distinct from natural number representations—evidence from a combined eye-tracking and computational modeling approach. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 172.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., Reips, U.-D., & Soltanlou, M. (2019). Individual differences influence two-digit number processing, but not their analogmagnitude processing: a large-scale online study. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1444–1464.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., Willmes, K., & Moeller, K. (2016). A general model framework for multisymbol number comparison. Psychological Review, 123(6), 667–695.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imbo, I., Vanden Bulcke, C., De Brauwer, J., & Fias, W. (2014). Sixty-four or four-and-sixty? The influence of language and working memory on children’s number transcoding. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 313.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, W., Nuerk, H.-C., Jäger, L., & Willmes, K. (2006). The influence of an external symbol system on number parity representation, or What’s odd about 6? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(4), 730–736.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iversen, W., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2004). Do signers think differently? The processing of number parity in deaf participants. Cortex, 40(1), 176–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kallai, A. Y., & Tzelgov, J. (2012). The place-value of a digit in multi-digit numbers is processed automatically. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(5), 1221–1233.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knops, A., Nuerk, H.-C., Sparing, R., Foltys, H., & Willmes, K. (2006). On the functional role of human parietal cortex in number processing: How gender mediates the impact of a ‘virtual lesion’induced by rTMS. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2270–2283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krajcsi, A., Lengyel, G., & Laczkó, Á. (2018). Interference between number magnitude and parity. Experimental Psychology, 65, 71–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krajcsi, A., & Szabó, E. (2012). The role of number notation: sign-value notation number processing is easier than place-value. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 463.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Macizo, P. (2017). Conflict resolution in two-digit number processing: evidence of an inhibitory mechanism. Psychological Research, 81(1), 219–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macizo, P., & Herrera, A. (2008). The effect of number codes in the comparison task of two-digit numbers. Psicológica, 29(1), 1–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macizo, P., Herrera, A., Román, P., & Martín, M. C. (2011). The processing of two-digit numbers in bilinguals. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 464–477.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerhoff, H. S., Moeller, K., Debus, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2012). Multi-digit number processing beyond the two-digit number range: A combination of sequential and parallel processes. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 81–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Huber, S., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2011). Two-digit number processing: holistic, decomposed or hybrid? A computational modelling approach. Psychological Research, 75(4), 290–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Klein, E., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2013). Magnitude representation in sequential comparison of two-digit numbers is not holistic either. Cognitive Processing, 14(1), 51–62.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2009). Internal number magnitude representation is not holistic, either. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(5), 672–685.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeller, K., Shaki, S., Göbel, S. M., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2015). Language influences number processing—a quadrilingual study. Cognition, 136, 150–155.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Bauer, F., Krummenacher, J., Heller, D., & Willmes, K. (2005a). The power of the mental number line: How the magnitude of unattended numbers affects performance in an Eriksen task. Psychology Science, 47(1), 34–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Iversen, W., & Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 57(5), 835–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Klein, E., & Willmes, K. (2013). Zahlenverarbeitung und Rechnen. In F. Schneider & G. R. Fink (Eds.), Funktionelle MRT in psychiatrie und neurologie (pp. 443–455). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., & Willmes, K. (2015). Multi-digit number processing: overview, conceptual clarifications, and language influences. In R. Cohen Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of numerical cognition (pp. 106–139). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2001). Decade breaks in the mental number line? Putting the tens and units back in different bins. Cognition, 82(1), B25–B33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., Weger, U., & Willmes, K. (2005b). Language effects in magnitude comparison: Small, but not irrelevant. Brain and Language, 92(3), 262–277.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representations of two-digit numbers. Psychology Science, 47(1), 52–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pixner, S., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., Nuerk, H.-C., & Kaufmann, L. (2011). Whorf reloaded: language effects on nonverbal number processing in first grade—A trilingual study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(2), 371–382.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2018). Modeling numerosity representation with an integrated diffusion model. Psychological Review, 125(2), 183–217.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliff, R., Thompson, C. A., & McKoon, G. (2015). Modeling individual differences in response time and accuracy in numeracy. Cognition, 137, 115–136.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ratinckx, E., Nuerk, H.-C., van Dijck, J.-P., & Willmes, K. (2006). Effects of interhemispheric communication on two-digit Arabic number processing. Cortex, 42(8), 1128–1137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, S., & Dixon, P. (2011). Repetition and the SNARC effect with one-and two-digit numbers. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 65(2), 84–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Rinsveld, A., Schiltz, C., Landerl, K., Brunner, M., & Ugen, S. (2016). Speaking two languages with different number naming systems: What implications for magnitude judgments in bilinguals at different stages of language acquisition? Cognitive Processing, 17(3), 225–241.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weis, T., Nuerk, H.-C., & Lachmann, T. (2018). Attention allows the SNARC effect to operate on multiple number lines. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 13778.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Neural representations of two-digit numbers: A parametric fMRI study. NeuroImage, 29(2), 358–367.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(4), 489–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1995). A representational analysis of numeration systems. Cognition, 57(3), 271–295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the participants. This research was funded by a DFG grant [NU 265/3-1] to HCN supporting KC and MS. KC, MS, and HCN are further supported by the LEAD Graduate School and Research Network [GSC1028], which is funded within the framework of the Excellence Initiative of the German federal and state governments. MS is also supported by the Institutional Strategy of the University of Tübingen (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, ZUK 63). Finally, we thank our assistants Florine Winkler, Lia Heubner and Marie-Lene Schlenker, who helped with data collection and Julianne Skinner for proofreading the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by a DFG grant [NU 265/3-1] to HCN supporting KC and MS.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Krzysztof Cipora.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix 1: The SNARC effect

In this additional analysis, we tested for the presence of the SNARC effect (Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes; Dehaene et al., 1993). This term refers to the observation that small magnitude numbers are responded to faster with left-side responses, whereas large magnitude numbers are responded to faster with right-side responses (see Fischer & Shaki, 2014 for a current review; Wood, Willmes, Nuerk, & Fischer, 2008 for a meta-analysis). The SNARC effect is considered as a behavioural signature of the semantic processing of numerical magnitude (Fias et al., 1996). The presence of the SNARC effect was evaluated using the method proposed by Fias et al. (1996). Namely, for each participant separately we calculated regression slopes where dRT (differences in right-hand–left-hand reaction times) were regressed on number magnitude. Slopes that are more negative represent a stronger SNARC effect. In the case of two-digit numbers, three types of SNARC effects can be calculated (Huber et al., 2016): (a) an overall SNARC effect—whole numerical magnitude considered, (b) a decade SNARC effect—decade number considered (collapsed across unit numbers), (c) a unit SNARC effect—unit number considered (collapsed across decade numbers). Each type of the SNARC slope was calculated separately for decade parity and unit parity condition. None of the SNARC slopes differed between English and German speakers (ts < 0.5, ps ≥ 0.630), thus the data were collapsed across language groups. Results are summarized in Table 5.

Table 5 SNARC slopes

A significant decade SNARC effect was found in the decade parity condition and a significant unit SNARC effect was found in the unit parity condition. The overall SNARC effect was only observed in the decade condition, but this might be due to a measurement artefact: decade digit largely determines the overall magnitude of a two-digit number. Note, however, that in principle, both decade and unit SNARC effects can be observed for multi-digit numbers in an appropriate setting (Weis et al., 2018).

Additionally, we calculated reliabilities of each SNARC effect using split-half method and adjusting for double test length with Spearman-Brown formula. Specifically, we adapted scripts described by (Cipora, van Dijck, et al., 2019), which are available at https://osf.io/n7szg/. In general, reliabilities for those conditions in which the SNARC effect was present were satisfactory and did not differ from values reported elsewhere in the literature. However, in those conditions in which SNARC was not significant, the reliabilities were very low or even negative. Please note that due to properties of Spearman-Brown formula, the negative values are more affected than positive ones: denominator of the formula is 1 + reliability. This is the reason why a relatively low negative correlation between halves (−0.30) was amplified to very low one of −0.86. This value is hardly interpretable (as any negative reliability estimate), and we present it for completeness only.

A 2 (Target digit: unit parity vs. decade parity) × 2 (SNARC type: unit SNARC vs. decade SNARC) repeated-measures ANOVA on mean SNARC slopes revealed no main effect of Target digit, F(1, 46) = 0.44, p = 0.510, η 2p  = 0.01, and no main effect of the SNARC type, F(1, 46) = 3.12, p = 0.084, η 2p  = 0.06. Crucially, there was a robust interaction between Target digit and SNARC type, F(1, 46) = 21.55, p < 0.001, η 2p  = 0.32. The SNARC was present only for target digits which were task-relevant (see Table 5 for descriptive results). An asymmetry was observed for the overall SNARC effect. It was only present in the decade parity condition. The overall SNARC slopes differed significantly between conditions, t(46) = −3.05, p = 0.004, d = 0.44.

These findings imply that spatial mapping can only be observed for numbers that were relevant in a given condition. The irrelevant numbers (unit number in the decade parity condition and decade number in the unit parity condition) did not evoke spatial mapping indexed by the SNARC effect.

In the last step of the analysis, we investigated correlations between slopes. In both conditions, we observed extremely strong correlations between decade SNARC slopes and overall SNARC slopes (rs > 0.92). This is due to the fact that decade magnitude plays a crucial role in overall number magnitude, thus these correlations are trivial. The overall SNARC slope in the unit condition correlated weakly with the unit SNARC slope from the unit condition (r = 0.32, p = 0.028). Interestingly, the two most robust SNARC effects, namely unit SNARC slope in the unit parity condition and the decade SNARC slope in the decade parity condition did not correlate with each other (r = 0.03, p = 0.859). None of the other correlations reached significance.

Appendix 2: Complete ANOVA model

Reaction times

Additionally, we conducted an ANOVA model considering all factors included in the design. Therefore, a 2 (Language: German vs. English, between subject) × 2 (Hand: right vs. left) × 2 (Parity of the target number: odd vs. even) × 2 (Congruency: congruent vs. incongruent) × 2 (Target digit: unit vs. decade) mixed-design ANOVA was conducted (cf. Table 6). Effects reported here reflect those reported in the main text, however, for the sake of completeness, we report them here again.

Table 6 The repeated measure ANOVA by Hand, Parity, Congruency, Target digit, and Language

Right-hand responses (539 ms, SD = 64 ms) were significantly quicker than left-hand responses (544 ms, SD = 64 ms; i.e., a main effect of hand). Responses to even numbers were significantly faster than responses to odd numbers (535 ms, SD = 63 ms and 547 ms, SD = 64 ms, respectively; i.e., a main effect of parity). Congruent trials were responded to quicker (533 ms, SD = 62 ms) than incongruent trials (549 ms, SD = 64 ms; i.e., a main effect of congruency). Reaction times did not differ depending on Target digit (i.e., no main effect of Target digit). Thus, we replicated effects typically observed in such setups, that is, the dominant hand advantage, the odd effect (Hines, 1990) as well as the parity congruency effect.

There was no main effect of language and this factor did not interact with any other factor (cf. Table 6), thus not confirming our predictions on linguistic influences on place-value processing.

As concerns the automatic activation of integrated parity, there was a significant Congruency × Target digit interaction (cf. Fig. 4, panel A). Incongruent trials were responded to significantly slower in the decade condition compared to the unit condition. To disambiguate this interaction, the congruency effect was calculated separately for each target digit by subtracting RTs incongruent—RTs congruent. Subsequently, the congruency effect was tested against zero with one-sample t-tests. In both conditions, decade and unit parity judgement, the congruency effect was highly significant (ps < 0.001). However, the congruency effect differed significantly between conditions, t(46) = 2.54, p = 0.014, d = 0.37 with a larger effect size in the decade condition than in the unit condition (Cohen’s d of 1.54 and 1.29, respectively). This is in line with the idea of automatic processing of integrated parity.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Panel a the Congruency × Condition interaction as reported in the main text. Panels b and c: Hand × Congruency × Condition interaction. Marginal means used, bars represent 95% confidence intervals

The significant Hand × Congruency × Target digit interaction indicated that the effect of interest differed depending on the responding hand. To disambiguate this three-way interaction, the Target digit × Congruency interactions were tested for each hand separately. No interaction was present in the case of left-hand responses, F(1,46) = 0.66, p = 0.420, η 2p  = 0.01 (cf. Fig. 4, panel B). For right-hand responses, this interaction was robust, F(1,46) = 15.02, p < 0.001, η 2p  = 0.25. A follow-up one-sample t test indicated that the congruency effect was robust in both conditions (ps < 0.001) for right hand responses, but the congruency effect differed significantly between conditions, t(46) = 3.88, p < 0.001, d = 0.5 (cf. Fig. 4, panel C) with a larger effect size for the decade condition than for the unit condition (Cohen’s d of 1.52 and 1.07, respectively).

Interestingly, there was also a significant Hand × Congruency interaction. As already suggested by the results of the three-way interaction described above, the congruency effect was more pronounced in right-hand responses compared to left-hand responses (cf. Fig. 4, panels B and C). For both hands the congruency effect was robust (ps < 0.001), however, the effect was more pronounced for right-hand responses than for left-hand responses (Cohen’s d of 1.73 and 1.59, respectively). The difference between conditions was also significant, t(46) = 2.04, p = 0.048, d = 0.30.

There was also a significant Parity × Congruency interaction, whereby even numbers were more affected by the congruency effect than odd numbers. The congruency effect was robust in both conditions (ps < 0.001, Cohen’s d of 1.68 and 1.64 for even and odd numbers, respectively) and the difference between the congruency effects for even and odd numbers was also significant, t(46) = 2.25, p = 0.029, d = 0.33.

The Hand × Parity interaction was not significant, thus revealing no MARC effect (see Nuerk et al., 2004). This interaction was also not modulated by the Target digit (non-significant three-way Hand × Parity × Target digit interaction). No other interactions reached significance.

Accuracies

Similar analysis for accuracies is summarized in Table 7.

Table 7 Full model, accuracy analysis

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cipora, K., Soltanlou, M., Smaczny, S. et al. Automatic place-value activation in magnitude-irrelevant parity judgement. Psychological Research 85, 777–792 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01268-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01268-1

Navigation