Skip to main content
Log in

Small numerosity advantage for sequential enumeration on RSVP stimuli: an object individuation-based account

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although there is a large literature demonstrating rapid and accurate enumeration of small sets of simultaneously presented items (i.e., subitizing), it is unclear whether this small numerosity advantage (SNA) can also manifest in sequential enumeration. The present study thus has two aims: to establish a robust processing advantage for small numerosities during sequential enumeration using a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm, and to examine the underlying mechanism for a SNA in sequential enumeration. The results indicate that a small set of items presented in fast sequences can be enumerated accurately with a high precision and a SOA (stimulus onset asynchrony)-sensitive capacity limit, essentially generalizing the large literature on small numerosity advantage from spatial domain to temporal domain. A resource competition hypothesis was proposed and confirmed in further experiments. Specifically, sequential enumeration and other cognitive process, such as visual working memory (VWM), compete for a shared resource of object individuation by which items are segregated as individual entities. These results implied that the limited resource of object individuation can be allocated within time windows of flexible temporal scales during simultaneous and sequential enumerations. Taken together, the present study calls for attention to the dynamic aspect of the enumeration process and highlights the pivotal role of object individuation in underlying a wide range of mental operations, such as enumeration and VWM.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note, to save space, we did not include the Weber fraction results in Experiment 1–3 which were basically similar to their respective VC results. The similarities between VC- and WF-based results in the present study can be observed in Experiment 4, see Fig. 8 left panel and Fig. 9 bottom right panel.

  2. Post hoc analyses in each ANOVA of each experiment were conducted using the Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

  3. Here, we used a low-end error of 5%, rather than 0%, to indicate a highly correct enumeration since other response-induced, enumeration-irrelevant random errors (no more than 5%, see Sagi & Julesz, 1985), e.g., pressing a wrong key accidentally even after correctly processing the stimuli, may also contribute to observed error rates.

References

  • Anobile, G., Arrighi, R., & Burr, D. C. (2019). Simultaneous and sequential subitizing are separate systems, and neither predicts math abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 178, 86–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anobile, G., Arrighi, R., Castaldi, E., Grassi, E., Pedonese, L., Moscoso, P. A. M., & Burr, D. C. (2018). Spatial but not temporal numerosity thresholds correlate with formal math skills in children. Developmental Psychology, 54, 458–475.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anobile, G., Castaldi, E., Turi, M., Tinelli, F., & Burr, D. C. (2016a). Numerosity but not texture density correlates with math ability in children. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1206–1216.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Anobile, G., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2016b). Number as a primary perceptual attribute: A review. Perception, 45(1), 25–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006615602599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anobile, G., Turi, M., Cicchini, G. M., & Burr, D. C. (2012). The effects of cross-sensory attentional demand on subitizing and on mapping number onto space. Vision Research, 74, 102–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. L. (1965). Flicker and intermittent stimulation. In C. H. Graham (Ed.), Vision and visual perception. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burr, D. C., Anobile, G., & Turi, M. (2011). Adaptation affects both high and low (subitized) numbers under conditions of high attentional load. Seeing and Perceiving, 24, 141–150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burr, D. C., Turi, M., & Anobile, G. (2010). Subitizing but not estimation of numerosity requires attentional resources. Journal of Vision, 10(6), 20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Camos, V., & Tillmann, B. (2008). Discontinuity in the enumeration of sequentially presented auditory and visual stimuli. Cognition, 107(3), 1135–1143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, X., Yang, Q., Han, Y., Ding, X., & Fan, Z. (2014). Capacity Limit of Simultaneous Temporal Processing: How Many Concurrent 'Clocks' in Vision? PLoS ONE, 9(3), e91797. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091797.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chesney, D. L., & Haladjian, H. H. (2011). Evidence for a shared mechanism used in multiple-object tracking and subitizing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(8), 2457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1980). The persistences of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 290(1038), 57–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cordes, S., Gelman, R., Gallistel, C. R., & Whalen, J. (2001). Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(4), 698–707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(18), 7426–7435.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H., & Pérusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 11, 561–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390–407.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drew, T., & Vogel, E. K. (2008). Neural measures of individual differences in selecting and tracking multiple moving objects. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 4183–4191.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, Z., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Singh, K. D., & Shapiro, K. (2012). The role of sustained posterior brain activity in the serial chaining of two cognitive operations: A MEG study. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1133–1144.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, Z., Singh, K. D., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Sigman, M., Dehaene, S., & Shapiro, K. (2011). The cost of serially chaining two cognitive operations. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0375-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307–314.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1991). Subitizing: The preverbal counting process. In F. Craik, W. Kessen, & A. Ortony (Eds.), Thoughts memories and emotions: Essays in honor of George Mandler (pp. 65–81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hecht, S., & Smith, E. L. (1936). Intermittent stimulation by light. VI. Area and the relation between critical frequency and intensity. Journal of General Physiology, 19, 978–989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, D. C., & Wood, J. N. (2011). Spatial attention determines the nature of nonverbal number representation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2336–2351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Calibrating the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 1221–1247.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jazayeri, M., & Shadlen, M. N. (2010). Temporal context calibrates interval timing. Nature Neuroscience, 13(8), 1020–1026.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jerome, E. A., & Keller, F. S. (1945). A test of two ‘‘remedial’’ devices in high-speed code reception. OSRD report, 5365.

  • Jevons, W. S. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3, 281–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzin, N., Cohen, Z. Z., & Henik, A. (2019). If it looks, sounds, or feels like subitizing, is it subitizing? A modulated definition of subitizing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(3), 790–797.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, E. L., Lord, M. W., Reese, T. W., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. The American Journal of Psychology, 62, 498–525.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2004). Beyond significance testing. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(2), 75–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Law, M. B., Pratt, J., & Abrams, R. A. (1995). Color-based inhibition of return. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 57(3), 402–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Cognitive processes in counting. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 310–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390, 279–281.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T. (1985). Use of numbers by a chimpanzee. Nature, 315(6014), 57–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazza, V. (2017). Simultanagnosia and object individuation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(7–8), 430–439.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLachlan, N. M., Marco, D. J. T., & Wilson, S. J. (2012). Pitch enumeration: Failure to subitize in audition. PLoS ONE, 7(4), e33661.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Meck, W. H., & Church, R. M. (1983). A mode control model of counting and timing processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 9, 320–334.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. https://doi.org/10.1038/2151519a0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieder, A. (2016). The neuronal code for number. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(6), 366–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nieder, A., Diester, I., & Tudusciuc, O. (2006). Temporal and spatial enumeration processes in the primate parietal cortex. Science, 313(8), 1431–1435.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olivers, C. N., & Watson, D. G. (2008). Subitizing requires attention. Visual Cognition, 16(4), 439–462.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagano, S., Lombard, L., & Mazza, V. (2014). Brain dynamics of attention and working memory engagement in subitizing. Brain Research, 1543, 244–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 437–442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, M., Fumarola, A., Chinello, A., & Melcher, D. (2011). Subitizing reflects visuo-spatial object individuation capacity. Cognition, 121(1), 147–153.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piazza, M., Giacomini, E., Bihan, D. L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). Single-trial classification of parallel pre-attentive and serial attentive processes using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings Biological Sciences, 270(1521), 1237.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306(5695), 499–503.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pincham, H. L., & Szűcs, D. (2012). Intentional subitizing: Exploring the role of automaticity in enumeration. Cognition, 124(2), 107–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556). NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vision, 3, 179–197.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2007). Perceiving the numerosity of rapidly occurring auditory events in metrical and nonmetrical contexts. Perception & Psychophysics, 69(4), 529–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revkin, S. K., Piazza, M., Izard, V., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Does subitizing reflect numerical estimation? Psychological Science, 19(6), 607–614.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohenkohl, G., Gould, I. C., Pessoa, J., & Nobre, A. C. (2014). Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. Journal of Vision, 14(4), 8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D., & Julesz, B. (1985). Detection versus discrimination of visual orientation. Perception, 14, 619–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Samuel, A. G., & Kat, D. (2003). Inhibition of return: A graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 897–906.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., McAvoy, M. P., dʼAvossa, G., & Corbetta, M. (2007). Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: Functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2625–2633.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55, 1233–1243.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., & Cooper, R. G. (1980). Perception of numbers by human infants. Science, 210, 1033–1035.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taubman, R. E. (1950). Studies in judged number: II. The judgment of visual number. The Journal of General Psychology, 43(2), 195–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. (1943). Report on a code aptitude test (privately printed).

  • Tobias, D. (1967). Number: The language of science (4th ed.). New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J., Fougnie, D., & Marois, R. (2005). Visual short-term memory load suppresses temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional blindness. Psychological Science, 16, 965–972.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, J., & Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 428(6984), 751–754.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 331–351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101(1), 80–102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vetter, P., Butterworth, B., & Bahrami, B. (2008). Modulating attentional load affects numerosity estimation: Evidence against a pre-attentive subitizing mechanism. PLoS One, 3(9), e3269.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vetter, P., Butterworth, B., & Bahrami, B. (2011). A candidate for the attentional bottleneck: Set-size specific modulation of the right TPJ during attentive enumeration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(3), 728–736.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, E. K., & Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature, 428, 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D. G., & Maylor, E. A. (2006). Effects of color heterogeneity on subitization. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 319–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, J., Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1999). Nonverbal counting in humans: The psychophysics of number representation. Psychological Science, 10, 130–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wutz, A., & Melcher, D. (2013). Temporal buffering and visual capacity: The time course of object formation underlies capacity limits in visual cognition. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 921–933.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wutz, A., & Melcher, D. (2014). The temporal window of individuation limits visual capacity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 952. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00952.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, Y., & Chun, M. M. (2009). Selecting and perceiving multiple visual objects. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13, 167–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, X., & Liu, C. (2008). Can subitizing survive the attentional blink? An ERP study. Neuroscience Letters, 440(2), 140–144.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was made possible by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31500869 and 31671122), China Scholarship Council (201806775014 and 201806775017) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, China (CCNU18TS037, CCNU19TS039, CCNU17TS025, CCNU19TS075 and CCNU19TD019).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors designed the study. XC and CL programmed the task and performed data analyses (along with CL). All authors contributed to manuscript preparation.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Xianfeng Ding or Zhao Fan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee, the American Psychological Association (APA) standards and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all the individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (RAR 48 kb)

Appendix

Appendix

Results of experiment 1d, 1e and 1f

Experiment 1d: 200–100 ms, Experiment 1e: 150–150 ms and Experiment 1f: 50–250 ms (all had 300 ms SOAs)

An ANOVA on percent error rate was conducted in Experiment 1d. A significant main effect of numerosity (Greenhouse–Geisser corrected) was found, F(3.08, 49.33) = 32.976, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.673. The classification criteria were the same as in Experiment 1c, i.e., all the numerosity levels were classified into a small numerosity range from 1 to 3 (ps > 0.99) and a large numerosity range from 4 to 10 (ps < 0.032). One sample t test demonstrated no significant difference between the mean error rate and the response-induced random error of 5% in the small numerosity range, t(16) = 0.058, p = 0.954, Cohen’s d = 0.014, mean = 0.051, SE = 0.017, but a significant difference in the large numerosity range, t(16) = 6.256, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.517, mean = 0.430, SE = 0.061. Meanwhile, the mean error rates in the small numerosities were significantly lower than those in the large numerosities, t(16) = − 7.135, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = − 1.998, mean difference = − 0.379, SE = 0.053. Analyses on AC demonstrated bias-free numerical reporting in the small numerosity range, t(16) = 1.247, p = 0.230, Cohen’s d = 0.302, mean = 0.009, SE = 0.007. However, the mean AC was significantly lower than 0 in the large numerosity range, t(16) = − 3.868, p < 0.002, Cohen’s d = − 0.938, mean = − 0.073, SE = 0.019, indicating a significant underestimation during serial subvocal verbal counting. A paired samples t test demonstrated a significantly lower mean VC, i.e., a higher precision, in the small numerosity than in the large numerosity, t(16) = − 2.115, p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.652, mean difference = − 0.032, SE = 0.015. The logistic fittings on error rates demonstrated a mean SNA capacity of 6.02 (SE = 0.56; average R2 = 0.92, SE = 0.02).

Experiment 1e showed similar results as Experiment 1c and 1d. A significant main effect of numerosity was found based on error rate data (Greenhouse–Geisser corrected), F(3.28, 52.41) = 66.327, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.806. With the same criteria as in Experiment 1c, all the numerosity levels were classified into a small numerosity range composed of level 1 to level 4 (ps > 0.10) and a large numerosity range composed of level 5 to level 10 (ps < 0.003). Analyses demonstrated no significant difference between the error rate in the small numerosity range and the response-induced random error (5%), t(16) = 1.482, p = 0.158, Cohen’s d = 0.359, mean = 0.088, SE = 0.026, but a significant difference between mean error rate in the large numerosity range and 5%, t(16) = 9.542, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 2.314, mean = 0.623, SE = 0.060. Meanwhile, the mean error rates in the small numerosities were significantly lower than those in the large numerosities, t(16) = − 11.581, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = − 2.717, mean difference = − 0.535, SE = 0.046. Analyses on AC and VC demonstrated a bias-free (t(16) = − 1.518, p = 0.149, Cohen’s d = − 0.368, mean = − 0.013, SE = 0.009), higher-precision (t(16) = − 3.254, p < 0.006, Cohen’s d = − 0.965, mean difference = − 0.038, SE = 0.012) enumeration processing in the small numerosity range and a significant underestimation (t(16) = − 5.821, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = − 1.412, mean = − 0.118, SE = 0.020) in the large numerosity range, which were all qualitatively in agreement with Experiment 1c and 1d. The logistic fittings on error rates demonstrated a mean SNA capacity of 5.33 (SE = 0.34; average R2 = 0.93, SE = 0.02).

Similar results were also found in Experiment 1f. A significant main effect of numerosity (Greenhouse–Geisser corrected), F(2.43, 41.23) = 38, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.691, was found for error rate data. Similar to Experiment 1c, Experiment 1f classified all the numerosity levels into two numerosity ranges with the same criteria, i.e., a small numerosity range from level 1 to level 3 (ps > 0.99) and a large numerosity range from level 4 to level 10 (ps < 0.05). Analyses on error rate demonstrated no significant difference between the mean error rate in the small numerosity and the response-induced random error (5%), t(17) = 0.37, p = 0.716, Cohen’s d = 0.087, mean = 0.056, SE = 0.018. However, the error rate in the large numerosity was significantly higher than the response-induced random error (5%), t(17) = 6.41, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.510, mean = 0.521, SE = 0.073. Meanwhile, the mean error rates in the small numerosities were significantly lower than those in the large numerosities, t(17) = − 7.260, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = − 1.987, mean difference = − 0.464, SE = 0.064. Analyses on AC and VC demonstrated bias-free (t(17) = − 0.869, p = 0.397, Cohen’s d = − 0.205, mean = − 0.007, SE = 0.008), higher-precision (t(17) = − 2.143, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = − 0.488, mean difference = − 0.025, SE = 0.012) enumeration processing in the small numerosity range and a significant underestimation (t(17) = − 3.823, p < 0.002, Cohen’s d = − 0.901, mean = − 0.127, SE = 0.033) in the large numerosity range. The logistic fittings on error rates demonstrated a mean SNA capacity of 5.82 (SE = 0.54; average R2 = 0.91, SE = 0.03).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cheng, X., Lin, C., Lou, C. et al. Small numerosity advantage for sequential enumeration on RSVP stimuli: an object individuation-based account. Psychological Research 85, 734–763 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01264-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01264-5

Navigation