Skip to main content
Log in

Flexibility of individual multitasking strategies in task-switching with preview: are preferences for serial versus overlapping task processing dependent on between-task conflict?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The prevalence and the efficiency of serial and parallel processing under multiple task demands are highly debated. In the present study, we investigated whether individual preferences for serial or overlapping (parallel) processing represent a permanent predisposition or depend on the risk of crosstalk between tasks. Two groups (n = 91) of participants were tested. One group performed a classical task switching paradigm, enforcing a strict serial processing of tasks. The second group of participants performed the same tasks in a task-switching-with-preview paradigm, recently introduced by Reissland and Manzey (2016), which in principle allows for overlapping processing of both tasks in order to compensate for switch costs. In one condition, the tasks included univalent task stimuli, whereas in the other bivalent stimuli were used, increasing risk of crosstalk and task confusion in case of overlapping processing. The general distinction of voluntarily occurring preferences for serial or overlapping processing when performing task switching with preview could be confirmed. Tracking possible processing mode adjustments between low- and high-crosstalk conditions showed that individuals identified as serial processors in the low-crosstalk condition persisted in their processing mode. In contrast, overlapping processors split up in a majority adjusting to a serial processing mode and a minority persisting in overlapping processing, when working with bivalent stimuli. Thus, the voluntarily occurring preferences for serial or overlapping processing seem to depend at least partially on the risk of crosstalk between tasks. Strikingly, in both crosstalk conditions the individual performance efficiency was the higher, the more they processed in parallel.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that, in the PRP literature the term “parallel” processing is used to describe concurrent processing exactly at the central stage of response selection. We prefer to use the term “overlapping” processing to refer to any partial or full concurrent processing of two tasks, which can include all processing stages.

  2. Note that the letters ‘D’, ‘P’, ‘T’, and ‘W’ are pronounced [deː], [peː], [teː], and [veː] in German, while the letters ‘H’, ‘K’, ‘J’, and ‘Q’ are pronounced [haː], [kaː], [jɔt], and [kuː].

  3. Note that we use the term “dual-tasking” for any situation where an individual has to cope with two tasks, including situations of real concurrent performance like in PRP-tasks, situations of classical task switching where individuals have to switch between two tasks, or situations of task switching which provide options of overlapping processing like in our TSWP paradigm.

References

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 769–786.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Gottschalk, C., & Dreisbach, G. (2014). Context-sensitive adjustment of cognitive control in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 399–416. doi:10.1037/a0034310.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Miller, J., & Schubert, T. (2007). Evidence for parallel semantic memory retrieval in dual tasks. Memory and Cognition, 35(7), 1685–1699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., & Plessow, F. (2015). Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1366. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01366.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2007). High perceptual load makes everybody equal: eliminating individual differences in distractibility with load. Psychological Science, 18, 377–382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2005). Comparison of four scoring methods for the reading span test. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 581–590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janczyk, M., Pfister, R., Hommel, B., & Kunde, W. (2014). Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance. Cognition, 132, 30–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 14, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. Variation in Working Memory, 1, 21–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874. doi:10.1037/a0019842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451–468.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 143–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., & Cox, S. (1997). On the efficiency of visual selective attention: efficient visual search leads to inefficient distractor rejection. Psychological Science, 8(5), 395–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehle, C., & Hübner, R. (2009). Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets. Psychological Research, 73, 707–726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehle, C., Steinhauser, M., & Hübner, R. (2009). Serial or parallel processing in dual tasks: what is more effortful? Psychophysiology, 46, 502–509.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Los, S. A. (1996). On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 145–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, R., & Meiran, N. (2005). Increased control demand results in serial processing evidence from dual-task performance. Psychological Science, 16(10), 833–840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N. (2010). Task switching: mechanisms underlying rigid vs. flexible self control. In R. R. Hassin, K. N. Ochsner, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Self control in society, mind and brain (pp. 202–220). New York: Oxford University.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research, 70, 484–493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., Ulrich, R., & Rolke, B. (2009). On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies. Cognitive Psychology, 58(3), 273–310. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller ST (2012) The Psychology Experiment Building Language, Version 0.13. Retrieved from http://pebl.sourceforge.net.

  • Mueller, S. T., & Piper, B. J. (2014). The Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 222, 250–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Miller, J. (1987). Role of outcome conflict in dual-task interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 435–448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 193–251. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plessow, F., Schade, S., Kirschbaum, C., & Fischer, R. (2012). Better not to deal with two tasks at the same time when stressed? Acute psychosocial stress reduces task shielding in dual-task performance. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(3), 557–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28, 164–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, J., & Manzey, D. (2016). Serial or overlapping processing in multitasking as individual preference: effects of stimulus preview on task switching and concurrent dual-task performance. Acta Psychologica, 168, 27–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salvucci, D. D. (2013). Multitasking. In J. D. Lee & A. Kirlik (Eds.), The oxford handbook of cognitive engineering (pp. 57–65). New York: Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, T., Fischer, R., & Stelzel, C. (2008). Response activation in overlapping tasks and the response-selection bottleneck. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 376–397.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T. L., Glass, J. M., Fencsik, D. E., Lauber, E. J., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12(2), 101–108. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spector, A., & Biederman, I. (1976). Mental set and mental shift revisited. The American Journal of Psychology, 89(4), 669–679. doi:10.2307/1421465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2002). All-or-none bottleneck versus capacity sharing accounts of the psychological refractory period phenomenon. Psychological Research, 66, 274–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2003). A central capacity sharing model of dualtask performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 3–18.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 498–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C. D. (1984). Processing resources in attention. In R. Parasuraman & R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 63–101). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3(2), 159–177. doi:10.1080/14639220210123806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grant DFG 3759/4-1 provided from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to the second author. Thanks are due to Jessika Reissland for helpful comments to an earlier draft of this article, to Marie Mückstein for collecting the data, and to Marcus Bleil for his technical support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jovita Brüning.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was funded by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG 3759/4-1).

Conflict of interest

Dietrich Manzey declares that he has no conflict of interest. Jovita Bruening declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix: Definition of overall dual-tasking performance efficiency (ODTPE)

Appendix: Definition of overall dual-tasking performance efficiency (ODTPE)

The ODTPE measure is a straightforward throughput measure. It describes how many of two tasks can be performed correctly in a given time when an individual has to cope with these two tasks concurrently or in close succession compared to a situation where the same tasks can be performed under single-task conditions.Footnote 3. We refer to overall benefits of dual-tasking if the overall performance of two tasks performed under dual-task conditions is better, that is more tasks are performed correctly, than what would theoretically be expected in case of strictly separated processing of the two component tasks without considering any dual-tasking costs. Costs of dual-tasking are assumed if overall dual-task performance is worse, that is less tasks are performed correctly, than what would be expected from strictly separated processing. This logic can be illustrated by the following Gedankenexperiment:

Assume an individual can correctly solve 90 trials of a letter classification task (task A), and 80 trials of a digit classification task (task B), both in a single-task block of 1 min each. If this individual then has to work on both tasks concurrently and/or in close succession for another 1-min block, we would theoretically expect 45 correct responses to the letter classification task and 40 correct responses to the digit classification task, given that (1) this individual works on the two tasks in a strict serial processing mode, (2) this individual is able to perform the different tasks with the same speed as under single-task conditions, and (3) neither general costs or benefits of dual-tasking arise. Considering the performance of the component tasks separately and comparing it to the respective single-task performance, this might suggest a performance decrement of 50% in the dual-task condition. However, considering the overall performance for both component tasks together, and the fact that the time available per task was cut by 50% in the dual-task condition, neither a loss nor a benefit in performance was produced. Thus, the throughput of tasks has actually remained the same for both conditions. Dual-task benefits would be reflected in any higher throughput, that is a total number of correctly performed tasks (summed across both component tasks) higher than 85. This, for example, might be possible if some overlapping processing of the two tasks takes place. In contrast, overall dual-task costs would be reflected in the fact that an individual would achieve a fewer overall number of correct responses than could have been expected from the single-task performance in the single-task blocks (< 85). This could be due to, for example, costs of task switching or costs related to outcome conflicts.

In our experiments, we worked with single-task blocks of 1 min and dual-task trials of 2 min. If a participant is working strictly serially (without considering any switch and mixing costs) we, thus, would expect the same number of correct responses in single-task and mixed-trials or dual-task blocks, respectively. Following this reasoning, we defined ODTPE formally as follows:

$${\text{ODTPE}} = 100\; \times \;\left[ {\frac{{\left( {{\text{nC}}_{{{\text{A\_dual}}}} + {\text{nC}}_{{{\text{B\_dual}}}} } \right)}}{{\left( {{\text{nC}}_{{{\text{A\_single}}}} + {\text{nC}}_{{{\text{B\_single}}}} } \right)}}} \right] - 100,$$

with nCA_single and nCB_sing le defined as number of correct responses in the respective tasks under single-task conditions, and nCA_dual and nCB_dual defined as the corresponding performance in dual-task conditions. Based on this measure, performance benefits of multitasking as described above are reflected in values of ODTPE > 0. In contrast, costs of multitasking are reflected in values of ODTPE < 0. Note that the consideration of the number of correct responses generates an overall efficiency measure that represents costs and benefits reflected in both, response times and error rates.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brüning, J., Manzey, D. Flexibility of individual multitasking strategies in task-switching with preview: are preferences for serial versus overlapping task processing dependent on between-task conflict?. Psychological Research 82, 92–108 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0924-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0924-0

Navigation