Skip to main content
Log in

The exogenous and endogenous control of attentional focusing

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Selective visual attention involves prioritizing both the location (orienting) and distribution (focusing) of processing. To date, much more research has examined attentional orienting than focusing. One of the most well-established findings is that orienting can be exogenous, as when a unique change in luminance draws attention to a spatial location (e.g., Theeuwes in Atten Percept Psychophys 51:599–606, 1992; Yantis and Jonides in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:601, 1984), and endogenous, as when a red distractor shape diverts attention when one is looking for a red target (e.g., Bacon and Egeth in Percept Psychophys 55:485–496, 1994; Folk et al. in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:1030, 1992). Here we ask whether attentional focusing—the broadening and contracting of prioritized processing—is influenced by the same two factors. Our methodology involved a dual-stream attentional blink task; participants monitored two spatially separated streams of items for two targets that could appear unpredictably either in the same stream or in opposite streams. The spatial distribution of attention was assessed by examining second-target accuracy in relation to inter-target lag and target location (same or opposite streams). In Experiment 1, we found that attentional contracting was more rapid when the targets differed in luminance from the distractor items. In Experiments 2 and 3, we found that the rate of attentional contracting was slower when there were task-relevant distractors in the stream opposite the first target. These results indicate that the rate of attentional focusing, like orienting, can be modulated by both exogenous and endogenous mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Several previous studies have provided evidence of such contracting of attention to the T1-stream (e.g., Jefferies et al., 2007, 2015; Jefferies & Di Lollo, 2009; Visser, Bischof, & Di Lollo, 1999). It is assumed that this contracting occurs primarily to optimize T1 identification accuracy. There is also evidence that T1 can be localized prior to being identified (Ghorashi, Jefferies, Kawahara, & Watanabe, 2008; Ghorashi, Enns, Klein, & Di Lollo, 2010), and that attention is deployed to the target’s location even when the target’s identity cannot be reported (Woodman & Luck, 2003).

  2. We thank Elkan Akyürek (personal communication, June 2017) for suggesting temporal integration as a principle on which to explain the present results.

References

  • Akyürek, E. G., Eshuis, S. A., Nieuwenstein, M. R., Saija, J. D., Başkent, D., & Hommel, B. (2012). Temporal target integration underlies performance at lag 1 in the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1448.

  • Awh, E., & Pashler, H. (2000). Evidence for split attentional foci. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 834–846.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 485–496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barriopedro, M. I., & Botella, J. (1998). New evidence for the zoom lens model using the RSVP technique. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1406–1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bay, M., & Wyble, B. (2014). The benefit of attention is not diminished when distributed over two simultaneous cues. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1287–1297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benso, F., Turatto, M., Mascetti, G. G., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The time course of attentional focusing. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 378–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Ehrenstein, A., Pritchard, K., Hiscock, M., & Crisan, J. (1999). The roles of location specificity and masking mechanisms in the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 61, 798–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breitmeyer, B. G., Hoar, W. S., Randall, D. J., & Conte, F. P. (1984). Visual masking: An integrative approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castiello, U., & Umiltà, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of processing. Acta Psychologica, 73, 195–209.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., & Potter, M. C. (1995). A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 109–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., Yantis, S., & Dechief, L. G. (2000). Response latencies to the onset and offset of visual stimuli. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 62, 218–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. (1977). Attention and preattention. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 11, 277–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Grubert, A. (2014). Spatial attention can be allocated rapidly and in parallel to new visual objects. Current Biology, 24, 193–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2008). Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1423–1433.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., & St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 225–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y-y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 583–597.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Paganoni, P., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., & Mascetti, G. G. (2003). The time course of attentional focusing in dyslexic and normally reading children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 181–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 1030.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Wright, J. H. (1994). The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 317.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghorashi, S., Enns, J. T., Klein, R. M., & Di Lollo, V. (2010). Spatial selection and target identification are separable processes in visual search. Journal of Vision, 10(3), 7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ghorashi, S. M. S., Jefferies, L. N., Kawahara, J.-I., & Watanabe, K. (2008). Does attention accompany the conscious awareness of both location and identity of an object. Psyche, 14, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghorashi, S. M., Zuvic, S. M., Visser, T. A., & Di Lollo, V. (2003). Focal distraction: spatial shifts of attentional focus are not required for contingent capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwood, P. M., Parasuraman, P., & Haxby, J. V. (1993). Changes in visuospatial attention over the adult lifespan. Neuropsychologia, 31, 471–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, H. L., Shafto, M. G., & Richardson, K. (1988). Effects of target luminance and cue validity on the latency of visual detection. Perception & Psychophysics, 44(5), 484–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., & Akyürek, E. G. (2005). Lag-1 sparing in the attentional blink: Benefits and costs of integrating two events into a single episode. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 58, 1415–1433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jans, B., Peters, J. C., & De Weerd, P. (2010). Visual spatial attention to multiple locations at once: The jury is still out. Psychological Review, 117, 637–684.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., & Di Lollo, V. (2009). Linear changes in the spatial extent of the focus of attention across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1020–1031.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., & Di Lollo, V. (2015). When can spatial attention be deployed in the form of an annulus? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 413–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., Enns, J. T., & Di Lollo, V. (2014a). The flexible focus: Whether spatial attention is unitary or divided depends on observer goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 465.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., Ghorashi, S., Kawahara, J-i, & Di Lollo, V. (2007). Ignorance is bliss: The role of observer expectation in dynamic spatial tuning of the attentional focus. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1162–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L. N., Gmeindl, L., & Yantis, S. (2014b). Attending to illusory differences in object size. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1393–1402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferies, L.N., & Witt, J.B. First unitary, then divided: The temporal dynamics of dividing attention (under review).

  • Jefferies, L. N., Roggeveen, A. B., Enns, J. T., Bennett, P. J., Sekuler, A. B., & Di Lollo, V. (2015). On the time course of attentional focusing in older adults. Psychological Research, 79, 28–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1983). Further toward a model of the mind’s eye’s movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21(4), 247–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawahara, J. I., & Yamada, Y. (2006). Two noncontiguous locations can be attended concurrently: Evidence from the attentional blink. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 594–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 371.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lit, A., Young, R. H., & Shaffer, M. (1971). Simple time reaction as a function of luminance for various wavelengths. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 10(6), 397–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lunau, R., & Olivers, C. N. (2010). The attentional blink and lag 1 sparing are nonspatial. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 317–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maringelli, F., & Umiltà, C. (1998). The control of the attentional focus. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 225–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMains, S. A., & Somers, D. C. (2004). Multiple spotlights of attentional selection in human visual cortex. Neuron, 42, 677–686.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, M. M., Malinowski, P., Gruber, T., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Sustained division of the attentional spotlight. Nature, 424, 309–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes, 32, 531–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, 240(4859), 1627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Sheese, B. E., Odludaş, Y., & Tang, Y. (2006). Analyzing and shaping human attentional networks. Neural Networks, 19, 1422–1429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, M. C., Chun, M. M., Banks, B. S., & Muckenhoupt, M. (1998). Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 979–992.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849–860.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, R. W., Johnston, J. C., & Yantis, S. (1992). Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 51, 279–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ronconi, L., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Molteni, M., & Facoetti, A. (2012). Zoom-out attentional impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Cortex, 49, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shih, S. I. (2000). Recall of two visual targets embedded in RSVP streams of distractors depends on their temporal and spatial relationship. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1348–1355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spalek, T. M., Lagroix, H. E., Yanko, M. R., & Di Lollo, V. (2012). Perception of temporal order is impaired during the time course of the attentional blink. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 402.

  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, T. A. W., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999a). Attentional switching in spatial and non-spatial domains: evidence from the attentional blink. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 458–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visser, T. A., Zuvic, S. M., Bischof, W. F., & Di Lollo, V. (1999b). The attentional blink with targets in different spatial locations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 432–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weichselgartner, E., & Sperling, G. (1987). Dynamics of automatic and controlled visual attention. Science, 238, 778–781.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2003). Serial deployment of attention during visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 121.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. (1954). Experimental Psychology. New Delhi: Oxford & IBH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, Y., & Kawahara, J. I. (2007). Dividing attention between two different categories and locations in rapid serial visual presentations. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 1218–1229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S. (1993). Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 676–681.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Discovery Grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to V. Di Lollo and J. T. Enns.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa N. Jefferies.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jefferies, L.N., Enns, J.T. & Di Lollo, V. The exogenous and endogenous control of attentional focusing. Psychological Research 83, 989–1006 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0918-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0918-y

Navigation