Skip to main content
Log in

Semantic alignment and number comparison

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Are the quantity representations activated by Arabic digits influenced by semantic context? We developed a novel paradigm to examine semantic alignment effects (e.g., Bassok et al. in J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 34:343–352, 2008) in number comparison. A horizontal word pair (either less more or few many) appeared for 480 ms to prime either relative magnitude (less more) or quantity (few many). Then a horizontal pair of single digits that were either successors (near) or differed by at least four (far) appeared above the word pair. Participants indicated verbally whether or not the word and digit pairs were congruent with respect to left-to-right ascending or descending relative magnitude. The RT advantage for far number pairs compared to near pairs (the distance effect) was greater with magnitude primes (81 ms) than quantity primes (17 ms), demonstrating a semantic alignment effect. This effect disappeared in Experiment 2 in which participants received identical stimuli but named the larger of the two digits and were free to ignore the primes. Nonetheless, mean RT in Experiment 2 was faster with prime and target pairs both ascending or both descending, but only with quantity primes. This prime-dependent order-congruity effect suggests that semantic alignment with respect to numerical order affected number comparison in Experiment 2. The results thereby demonstrate that number comparison exhibits task-dependent semantic alignment effects and recruits distinct numerical representations as a function of semantic context (e.g., Cohen Kadosh and Walsh in Behav Brain Sci 32:313–373, 2009).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We explored analyses that treated reported use of the strategy “anchor on many or more” (n = 12) versus all other reported strategies (n = 12) as a between-participants factor. These analyses did not present any significant effects associated with this strategy classification.

References

  • Bassok, M. (2001). Semantic alignments in mathematical word problems. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (Chapter 12, 401–433). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., Chase, V., & Martin, S. (1998). Adding apples and oranges: Alignment of semantic and formal knowledge. Cognitive Psychology, 35, 99–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bassok, M., Pedigo, S., & Oskarsson, A. (2008). Priming addition facts with semantic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Language, Memory and Cognition, 34, 343–352. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D., & Metcalfe, A. W. S. (2009). Numerical abstractness and elementary arithmetic. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 32, 330–331. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D. (2009). Integers do not automatically activate their quantity representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 332–336. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.2.332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., & Walsh, V. (2009). Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: Abstract or not abstract. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 32, 313–373. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2003). The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 145–147. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00055-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research, 65, 250–259. doi:10.1007/s004260100065.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • French, P. L. (1979). Linguistic marking, strategy, and affect in syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 425–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganor-Stern, D., & Tzelgov, J. (2008). Across-notation automatic numerical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 430–437. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, U., & Glucksberg, S. (2006). Does understanding negation entail affirmation? An examination of negated metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1015–1032. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2005.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarmasz, J., & Hollands, J. G. (2009). Confidence intervals in repeated-measures designs: The number of observations principle. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 124–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jou, J. (2003). Multiple number and letter comparison: Directionality and accessibility in numeric and alphabetic memories. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 543–579. doi:10.2307/1423660.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeFevre, J., & Kulak, A. (1994). Individual differences in the obligatory activation of addition facts. Memory & Cognition, 22, 188–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeFevre, J., Bisanz, J., & Mrkonjic, L. (1988). Cognitive arithmetic: evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts. Memory & Cognition, 16, 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leth-Steensen, C., & Marley, A. A. J. (2000). A model of response-time effects in symbolic comparison. Psychological Review, 107, 62–100. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520. doi:10.1038/2151519a0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Opstal, F., Gevers, W., De Moor, W., & Verguts, T. (2008). Dissecting the symbolic distance effect: Comparison and priming effects in numerical and nonnumerical orders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 419–425. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.2.419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spellman, B. A., Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (2001). Analogical priming via semantic relations. Memory & Cognition, 29, 383–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turconi, E., Campbell, J. I. D., & Seron, X. (2006). Numerical order and quantity processing in number comparison. Cognition, 98, 273–285. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.12.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972). Psychology of reasoning: structure and content. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wisniewski, E., & Bassok, M. (1999). What makes a man similar to a tie? Stimulus compatibility with comparison and integration. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 208–238. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., Stoianov, I., & Umiltà, C. (2005). Computational modeling of numerical cognition. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 67–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. We thank Arron Metcalfe for helpful comments on an earlier version of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jamie I. D. Campbell.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Campbell, J.I.D., Sacher, S.G. Semantic alignment and number comparison. Psychological Research 76, 119–128 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0331-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0331-x

Keywords

Navigation