Advertisement

Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 205, Issue 4, pp 619–627 | Cite as

A magnetic compass guides the direction of foraging in a bat

  • Lanxiang TianEmail author
  • Bingfang Zhang
  • Jinshuo Zhang
  • Tongwei Zhang
  • Yao Cai
  • Huafeng Qin
  • Walter Metzner
  • Yongxin Pan
Original Paper

Abstract

Previously, two studies have provided evidence that bats can use magnetic field cues for homing or roosting. For insectivorous bats, it is well established that foraging represents one of the most fundamental behaviors in animals relies on their ability to echolocate. Whether echolocating bats can also use magnetic cues during foraging remains unknown, however. Here, we tested the orientation behavior of Chinese noctules (Nyctalus plancyi) during foraging in a plus-shaped, 4-channel apparatus under different magnetic field conditions. To minimize the effects of spatial memory on orientation from repeated experiments, naïve bats were tested only once in each experimental condition. As expected, under geomagnetic field and a food resource offered conditions, the bats significantly preferred to enter the channel containing food, indicating that they primarily relied on direct sensory signals unrelated to magnetic cues. In contrast, when we offered food simultaneously in all four channels and minimized any differences in all other sensory signals available, the bats exhibited a clear directional preference to forage along the magnetic field direction under either geomagnetic field or a magnetic field in which the horizontal component was rotated by 90°. Our study offers a novel evidence for the importance of a geomagnetic field during foraging.

Keywords

Geomagnetic field Bats Foraging orientation A plus-shaped channel apparatus Magnetic orientation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr. Wei Lin for helpful discussions. This study was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Numbers 41674071, 41621004) and the Chinese Academy of Sciences project (QYZDJ-SSW-DQC024).

Author contributions

LT, WM, and YP conceived the experiments and wrote the paper; BZ and JZ collected the animals and performed the experiments; HQ adjusted the experimental magnetic fields; and TZ and YC analyzed the behavioral data. All authors reviewed the manuscript and approved the final version submitted.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

The Ethics Committee of the Chinese Academy of Sciences on Vertebrate Animals Experiments and the Institute of Geology and Geophysics Administrative Panel on Animal Care approved all experimental procedures. The collection of the bats was approved by the Institute of Geology and Geophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences.

Supplementary material

359_2019_1353_MOESM1_ESM.docx (98 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 97 kb)

References

  1. Bell G (1985) The sensory basis of prey location by the California leaf-nosed bat Macrotus californicus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:343–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Carter GG, Ratcliffe JM, Galef BG (2010) Flower bats (Glossophaga soricina) and fruit bats (Carollia perspicillata) rely on spatial cues over shapes and scents when relocating food. PLoS One 5:e10808.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010808 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Cresci A, Paris CB, Durif CMF, Shema S, Bjelland RM, Skiftesvik AB, Browman HI (2017) Glass eels (Anguilla anguilla) have a magnetic compass linked to the tidal cycle. Sci Adv 3:e1602007CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Danchin E, Giraldeau L, Cezilly F (2008) Behavioural ecology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  5. Dubrov AP (1978) The geomagnetic field and life.Geomagnetobiology. Springer, New York, p 318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Firester AH (1996) Design of square Helmholtz coil systems. Rev Sci Instrum 37:1264–1265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  8. Hessel K, Schmidt U (1994) Multimodal orientation in Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae). Folia Zool 43(4):339–346Google Scholar
  9. Holland RA, Thorup K, Vonhof MJ, Cochran WW, Wikelski M (2006) Bat orientation using Earth’s magnetic field. Nature 444:702CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Holland RA, Kirschvink JL, Doak TG, Wikelski M (2008) Bats use magnetite to detect the Earth’s magnetic field. PLoS One 3(2):e1676.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001676 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Holland RA, Borissov I, Siemers BM (2010) A nocturnal mammal, the greater mouse-eared bat, calibrates a magnetic compass by the sun. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:6941–6945CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Jones PL, Page RA, Ratcliffe JM (2016) To scream or to listen? Prey detection and discrimination in animal-eating bats. In: Fenton MB, Grinnell AD, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Bat bioacoustics. Springer, New York, pp 93–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Li TT (2004) Tri-axial square Helmholtz coil for neutron EDM experiment. Chinese University of Hong Kong, p 23Google Scholar
  14. Metzner W, Müller R (2016) Ultrasound production, emission, and reception. In: Fenton MB, Grinnell AD, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Bat bioacoustics. Springer, New York, pp 55–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Mora CV, Acerbi ML, Bingman VP (2014) Conditioned discrimination of magnetic inclination in a spatial-orientation arena task by homing pigeons (Columba livia). J Exp Biol 217:4123–4131CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Muchhala N, Serrano D (2015) The complexity of background clutter affects nectar bat use of flower odor and shape cues. PLoS One 10:e0136657CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Rose A, Kolar M, Tschapka M, Knornschild M (2016) Learning where to feed: the use of social information in flower-visiting pallas’ long-tongued bats (Glossophaga soricina). Anim Cogn 19:251–262CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Rydell J, Eklöf J (2003) Vision complements echolocation in an aerial-hawking bat. Naturwissenschaften 90:481–483CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Schmidt U, Joermann G, Rother G (1988) Acoustical vs. visual orientation in neotropical bats. In: Nachtigall PE, Moore PWB (eds) Animal sonar processes and performance. Plenum Press, New York and London, pp 589–594CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Schnitzler HU, Kalko EKV (2001) Echolocation by insect-eating bats. Bioscience 51:557–569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Schnitzler HU, Moss CF, Denzinger A (2003) From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol Evol 18:386–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Speakman JR, Anderson ME, Racey PA (1989) The energy cost of echolocation in pipistrelle bats. J Comp Physiol A 165:670–685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Thalau P, Holtkamp-Rotzler E, Fleissner G, Wiltschko W (2007) Homing pigeons (Columba livia f. domestica) can use magnetic cues for locating food. Naturwissenschaften 94:813–819CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Thiele J, Winter Y (2005) Hierarchical strategy for relocating food targets in flower bats: spatial memory versus cue-directed search. Anim Behav 69:315–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tian LX, Lin W, Zhang SY, Pan YX (2010) Bat head contains soft magnetic particles: evidence from magnetism. Bioelectromagnetics 31:499–503CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Walker MM, Kirschvink JL, Chang SBR, Dizon AE (1984) A candidate magnetic sense organ in the yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares. Science 224:751–753CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Walker MM, Diebel CE, Kirschvink JL (2003) Detection and use the Earth’s magnetic field by aquatic vertebrates. In: Collin SP, Marshall NJ (eds) Sensory processing in aquatic environments. Springer, New York, pp 53–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Wang YN, Pan YX, Parsons S, Walker M, Zhang SY (2007) Bats respond to polarity of a magnetic field. Proc R Soc B 274:2901–2905CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Wegner RE, Begall S, Burda H (2006) Magnetic compass in the cornea: local anaesthesia impairs orientation in a mammal. J Exp Biol 209:4747–4750CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (1995) Magnetic orientation in animals. Springer, BerlinCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (2005) Magnetic orientation and magnetoreception in birds and other animals. J Comp Physiol A 191:675–693CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Key Laboratory of Earth and Planetary Physics, Institute of Geology and GeophysicsChinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  2. 2.Institutions of Earth ScienceChinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  3. 3.France-China International Laboratory of Evolution and Development of Magnetotactic Multicellular OrganismsChinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  4. 4.University of Chinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  5. 5.National Zoological Museum, Institute of ZoologyChinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  6. 6.PGL, Institute of Geology and GeophysicsChinese Academy of SciencesBeijingChina
  7. 7.Department of Integrative Biology and PhysiologyUniversity of California Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations