Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Maternity leave and children’s cognitive and behavioral development

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Population Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigate the impact of maternity leave on the cognitive and behavioral development of children at ages 4 and 5, following up previous research on these children at younger ages. The impact is identified by legislated increases in the duration of maternity leave in Canada, which significantly increased the amount of first-year maternal care. Our results indicate no positive effect on indices of children’s cognitive and behavioral development. We uncover a small negative impact on PPVT scores.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The “findings” preamble to the American Family and Medical leave Act states “…it is important for the development of children and the family unit that fathers and mothers be able to participate in early childrearing…” (Public Law No. 103-3 §2(a)(2). http://www.dol.gov/whd/fmla/fmlaAmended.htm) A recent Australian paid parental leave program was promoted with the claim that “the scheme will give more babies the best start in life. The payment will enable more parents to stay at home to care for their baby full-time during the vital early months of social, cognitive and physical development” (Commonwealth of Australia 2009). An extension of paid maternity leave in the United Kingdom seeks to “…give children the best start in life…” as the “…evidence confirms the value of consistent one-to-one care in the first year of a child’s life.” (Employment Relations Directorate 2006, p. 2).

  2. See Baker and Milligan (2008b, 2010) and Hanratty and Trzcinski (2009).

  3. For example, Ruhm (2004) reports reductions in PPVT scores of 7–8 % of a standard deviation from maternal employment in the first year while Bernal (2008) reports that a full year of full-time maternal employment in the first 5 years of life reduces test scores by 0.13 of standard deviation (PPVT and the Peabody Individual Achievement Tests). This research is reviewed in Lucas-Thompson et al. (2010).

  4. For example, O’Brien Caughy et al. (1994) report that entrance into daycare before the first birthday was associated with higher test scores (Peabody Individual Achievement Tests) for lower income children and lower test scores for higher income children. For the UK, Gregg et al. (2005) find that children who receive informal care from friends and relatives in the first 18 months of life combined with full time maternal employment have lower cognitive outcomes. In the Canadian context, Lefebvre et al. (2008) report that Quebec’s universal, low fee childcare program, which serves children from birth, is related to reductions in PPVT scores of just under one-third of a standard deviation. Finally, Loeb et al. (2007) find that entry into non-parental center based care before the age of one can lead to problem behavior. Magnuson et al. (2007), Baker et al. (2008), and Datta Gupta and Simonsen (2010), and the research summarized in Belsky (2006) provide further evidence that non-parental care can have negative behavioral effects in some contexts.

  5. Historically, mothers have taken the vast majority of the leave, although this is (slowly) changing in recent years. Marshall (2008) reports that in 2006, 23 % of eligible fathers took some parental leave.

  6. The changes in provincial mandates were from 29–35 to 52–54 weeks with the exceptions of Alberta, where the change was from 18 to 52 weeks, and Quebec, where the entitlement did not change from a level of 70 weeks.

  7. Two provinces, Saskatchewan and Alberta, did not change their job protected leave standards until 2001. Unfortunately there are not sufficient observations from these provinces over the 2–6 months of delay to take advantage of this feature of the reform.

  8. The scale is: 0—the child has not reached the predimensional level, 1—the child has reached the predimensional level (4-year-old equivalent), 2—the child has reached the unidimensional level (6-year-old equivalent) and 3—the child has reached the bidimensional level (8-year-old equivalent).

  9. Public childcare changes contemporaneous with the parental leave reform could also influence child outcomes. We are not aware of any other provinces with childcare changes over this time period.

  10. The proportion of children age 0–5 living with two parents in 2000 in the Labour Force Survey is 92.2 %. Single-parenthood is much less prevalent in Canada than the USA.

  11. Alberta and Saskatchewan did not change their maternity leave provisions to match the change in the federal EI rules until after December 2000. We therefore also exclude the very small number of children born in Alberta and Saskatchewan in the months between December 2000 and the point when the provincial maternity leave mandate changed a few months later.

  12. Age-standardized PPVT scores are available in the NLSCY as provided by Statistics Canada. The age-standardization is based on the scores of respondents in the first five cycles of the Survey. Details are reported in Statistics Canada (n.d.). Age-standardized scores are not provided for the behavioral or other cognitive measures. For these measures we specify a full set of age (in months) and month of birth effects as additional control variables.

  13. The substitution of TFY i for T i provides a neat solution for observations with censored values of T i by limiting the time frame to 1 year. For example, for children surveyed at older ages, the observation of T i may be censored because the mother is still at home at the date of the survey.

  14. The regressions for age-standardized PPVT scores omit the month of birth and single month of child’s age. The results including these controls are reported in footnotes below. We control for place of birth to account for any differences in immigration patterns. Over this time period, however, there were no strong shifts in the class or source of immigration to Canada. Robust standard errors reported for all estimates. These standard errors are “conservative,” in the sense that the standard errors clustering on province, year of birth or a pre-/post-policy reform indicator are generally smaller.

  15. The polynomials in quarter are not included here given the specification of year of birth effects.

  16. Adding controls for the child’s age and month of birth the estimate is −0.415 (0.486). As noted below (i.e., footnote 20) this change in the estimate is due to the sensitivity of the estimate for girls to the change in the specification of the age and month of birth effects.

  17. The second stage collapses when we specify a quintic in time. The model is not identified when we use quarter of birth dummy variables since they perfectly predict the instrument.

  18. The number of observations varies across the different outcomes. Restricting the sample to children with valid observations on all outcomes leads to similar estimates.

  19. The exception is hyperactivity—the estimates are negative, quite large and in some specifications statistically significant.

  20. Controlling for child’s age and month of birth the estimate for boys is −1.443 (0.743) and for girls, 0.953 (0.672).

  21. The estimates for the 8 year of birth effects are reported in Baker and Milligan (2011).

  22. Cycles 6–8 are the source of information on the non parental care of post reform children at these older ages. These sampling issues make the comparison of these responses to the responses of pre reform cohorts from earlier waves problematic. The issues include a computer glitch that led to missing values for one-quarter of children in cycle 6.

  23. We investigated the potential impact of differential fertility on the composition of our sample by looking at the fraction of childless couples, the average number of children among those with children, and the fraction of those having a child and who are married. No differences in the trend after 2000/2001 are observed.

  24. As noted earlier, the year of birth is not directly reported in the LFS. Instead, we identify the year of birth for children by selecting a sample of children in December of each year. The regressions also include controls for province, urban/rural residence, mother’s and spouse’s age and education.

  25. We create real earnings by converting the earnings reports to 2002 dollars using the Consumer Price Index. In the presence of a general upward trend in wages across years, our estimates here based on just the time series variation will attribute to the policy what is really just a trend in real wage growth. We have also re-estimated these regressions deflating earnings by the growth in the Industrial Aggregate Wage from Statistics Canada’s Survey of Employment Payrolls and Hours (catalogue 72-002-XIB). Using these wage-growth-adjusted earnings tells a somewhat different story. They indicate a smaller (by half) increase in family earnings at ages 13–35 months, and little increase at older ages. These results are reported in Baker and Milligan (2011).

  26. The LFS does not provide any direct information on whether the child is in non-parental care.

  27. Our brief discussion of these milestones follows Scher and Harel (2009).

References

  • Ahnert L, Gunnar MR, Lamb ME, Barthel M (2004) Transition to child care: associations with infant–mother attachment, infant negative emotion, and cortisol elevations. Child Dev 75(3):639–650

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Almond D, Currie J (2011) Human capital development before age five. In: Card D, Ashenfelter O (eds) Handbook of labor economics, vol 4b. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 1315–1486

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker M (2011) Universal early childhood interventions: what is the evidence base? Can J Econ 44(4):1069–1105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker M, Milligan K (2008a) Maternal employment, breastfeeding, and health: evidence from maternity leave mandates. J Health Econ 27(4):871–887

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker M, Milligan K (2008b) How does job-protected maternity leave affect mothers’ employment? J Labor Econ 26(4):655–691

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker M, Milligan K (2010) Evidence from maternity leave expansions of the impact of maternal care on early child development. J Hum Resour 45(1):1–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker M, Milligan K (2011) Maternity leave and children’s cognitive and behavioral development. NBER working paper #17105

  • Baker M, Gruber J, Milligan K (2008) Universal childcare, maternal labor supply, and family well-being. J Polit Econ 116(4):709–745

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Belsky J (2006) Early child care and early child development: major findings of the NICHD study of early child care. Eur J Dev Psychol 3(1):95–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal R (2008) The effect of maternal employment and child care on children’s cognitive development. Int Econ Rev 49(4):1173–1209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal R, Keane MP (2011) Child care choices and children’s cognitive achievement: the case of single mothers. J Labor Econ 29(3):459–512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley-Johnson S (2001) Cognitive assessment for the youngest children: a critical review of tests. J Psychoeduc Assess 19:19–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks-Gunn J, Han W-J, Waldfogel J (2002) Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: the NICHD study of early child care. Child Dev 73(4):1052–1072

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carneiro P, Løken KV, Salvanes KG (2011) A flying start? Maternity benefits and long run outcomes of children. IZA discussion paper no. 5793

  • Case R, Okamoto Y, Griffin S, McKeough A, Bleiker C, Henderson B, Stephenson KM, Siegler RS, Keating DP (1996) The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children’s thought. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 61:1–2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterji P, Markowitz S (2005) Does the length of maternity leave affect maternal health? South Econ J 72(1):16–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Commonwealth of Australia (2009) Australia’s paid parental leave scheme. CanPrint Communications Pty Ltd

  • Danzer N, Lavy V (2013) Parental leave and medium-run cognitive child outcomes: quasi-experimental evidence from a large parental leave reform. NBER Working Paper Number 19452

  • Datta Gupta N, Simonsen M (2010) Non-cognitive child outcomes and universal high quality child care. J Public Econ 94(1–2):30–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Lemos M, Doig B (1999) Who am I? Developmental assessment manual. Australian Council for Educational Research, Melbourne

    Google Scholar 

  • Dustmann C, Schonberg U (2012) Expansions in maternity leave coverage and children’s long-term outcomes. Am Econ J Appl Econ 4(3):190–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Employment Relations Directorate (2006) The maternity and parental leave (amendment) regulations 2006 and the paternity and adoption leave (amendment) regulations 2006, full regulatory impact assessment. Department of Trade and Industry, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregg P, Washbrook E, Propper C, Burgess S (2005) The effects of a mother’s return to work decision on child development in the UK. Econ J 115(501):F48–F80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar M, Quevedo K (2007) The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu Rev Psychol 58:145–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nahratty M, Trzcinski E (2009) Who benefits from paid family leave? Impact of expansions in Canadian paid family leave on maternal employment and transfer income. J Popul Econ 22(3):693–711

  • Klerman JA, Leibowitz A (1997) Labor supply effects of state maternity leave legislation. In: Blau FD, Ehrenberg RG (eds) Gender and family issues in the workplace. Russell Sage, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefebvre P, Merrigan P, Verstraete M (2008) Childcare policy and cognitive outcomes of children: results from a large scale quasi-experiment on universal childcare in Canada. CIRPEE, Cahier de recherche/working paper 08–23

  • Liu Q, Skans ON (2010) The duration of paid parental leave and children’s scholastic performance. B E J Econ Anal Policy Contrib 10(1), article 3

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb S, Bridges M, Bassok D, Fuller B, Rumberger RW (2007) How much is too much? The influence of preschool centers on children’s social and cognitive development. Econ Educ Rev 26(1):52–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas-Thompson RG, Goldberg WA, Prause JA (2010) Maternal work early in the lives of children and its distal association with achievement and behavior problems: a meta analysis. Psychol Bull 136(6):915–942

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magnuson KA, Ruhm C, Waldfogel J (2007) Does prekindergarten improve school preparation and performance. Econ Educ Rev 26(1):33–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall K (2008) Father’s use of paid parental leave. Perspect Labour Income 20:5–14

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien Caughy M, DiPietro J, Strobino D (1994) Daycare participation as a protective factor in the cognitive development of low-income children. Child Dev 65:457–471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen AW (2010) Increasing the length of parents’ birth-related leave: the effect on children’s long-term educational outcomes. Labour Econ 17(1):91–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruhm CJ (2004) Parental employment and child cognitive development. J Hum Resour 39(1):155–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saye KB (2003) Preschool intellectual assessment. In: Reynolds CR, Kamphaus RW (eds) Handbook of psychological and educational assessment of children, 2nd edn. Guilford, New York, pp 187–203

    Google Scholar 

  • Scher A, Harel J (2009) Separation and stranger anxiety. In: Benson JB, Haith MM (eds) Social and emotional development in infancy and early childhood. Academic, Oxford, pp 380–389

    Google Scholar 

  • Schonberg U, Ludsteck J (2014) Expansions in maternity leave coverage and mothers’ labor market outcomes after childbirth. J Labor Econ 32(3):469–505

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Canada (2006) Employment insurance coverage survey. The Daily, Nov. 1st

  • Statistics Canada (n.d.) Microdata user guide, national longitudinal survey of children and youth, cycle 8, September 2008 to July 2009. http://www23.statcan.gc.ca/imdb-bmdi/document/4450_D4_T9_V8-eng.pdf. Accessed 19 Aug 2014

  • Waldfogel J (2006) What children need. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We have benefited from comments from Josh Angrist and seminar participants at the 2011 AEA meetings, UC Davis, Laval, Manitoba, Stavanger, Texas, W.E. Upjohn and Waterloo. We also thank the referees for helpful suggestions. We gratefully acknowledge the research support of SSHRC (Baker Grant, #410-2008-0346, #410-2011-0724 Milligan Grant #410-2006-0928). We thank the staffs of the Toronto and B.C. Research Data Centres for their technical support. This paper represents the views of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the views of Statistics Canada.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Baker.

Additional information

Responsible Editor: Alessandro Cigno

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baker, M., Milligan, K. Maternity leave and children’s cognitive and behavioral development. J Popul Econ 28, 373–391 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-014-0529-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-014-0529-5

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation