Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Understanding the link between the economy and teenage sexual behavior and fertility outcomes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Population Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We use individual-level data from the 1997 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth and state unemployment rates to examine how the economy affects fertility and its proximate determinants for several groups based on gender, age (15–17 and 18–20 groups), and race/ethnicity. We find that, for 15- to 17-year-old females, several behaviors leading to pregnancies and pregnancies themselves are higher when the unemployment rate is higher, which is consistent with the counter-cyclical fertility patterns for this group. For 18- to 20-year-old males, the results suggested counter-cyclical patterns of fertility behaviors/outcomes for whites, but pro-cyclical patterns for blacks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arkes J (2007) Does the economy affect teenage substance use? Health Econ 16(1):19–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertrand M, Duflo E, Mullainathan S (2004) How much should we trust differences-in-differences estimates? Q J Econ 119(1):249–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butz WP, Ward MP (1979) The emergence of countercyclical U.S. fertility. Am Econ Rev 69(3):318–328

    Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain G (1984) Panel data. In: Griliches Z, Intrilligator M (eds) Handbook of econometrics, vol 2. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 1247–1318

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper ML, Shapiro CM, Powers AM (1998) Motivations for sex and risky sexual behavior among adolescents and young adults: a functional perspective. J Pers Soc Psychol 75(6):1528–1558

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hotz JV, Klerman JA, Willis R (1997) The economics of fertility in developed countries. In: Rosenzweig R, Stark O (eds) Handbook of population and family economics, vol 1A. Elsevier, North-Holland, pp 275–347

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones E, Forest J (1992) Underreporting of abortions in surveys of U.S. Women: 1976–1988. Demography 29(1):113–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klerman JA, Danielson C (2004) Why did the welfare caseload decline? RAND Working Paper, WR-167

  • Krämer W, Neusser K (1984) The emergence of countercyclical U.S. fertility. Am Econ Rev 74(1):201–202

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine PB (2001) The sexual activity and birth-control use of American teenagers. In: Gruber J (ed) Risky behavior among youths: an economic analysis. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 167–217

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine PB (2002) The impact of social policy and economic activity throughout the fertility decision tree. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper No. 9021.

  • Macunovich DJ (1995) The butz-ward fertility model in the light of more recent data. The J Hum Resour 30(2):229–255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin J, Hamilton B, Sutton P, Ventura SJ, Menacker F, Munson M (2003) Births: final data for 2002. National Vital Statistics System 52(10)

  • McDonald J (1983) The emergence of countercyclical U.S. fertility: a reassessment of the evidence. J Macroecon 5(4):421–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulton B (1990) An illustration of a pitfall in estimating the effects of aggregate variables on micro units. Rev Econ Stat 72(2):334–338

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Office of the President of the United States (2004) Economic report of the president. Government Printing Office, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Sen B (2002) Does alcohol-use increase the risk of sexual intercourse among adolescents? Evidence from the NLSY97. J Health Econ 21(6):1085–1093

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ventura S, Abma J, Mosher WD, Henshaw S (2004) Estimated pregnancy rates for the United States, 1990–2000: an Update. National Vital Statistics System 52(23)

Download references

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank Phillip Levine for providing data on abortion laws, Paul Steinberg for editorial assistance, Chris Dirks for formatting assistance, and the helpful comments from two anonymous referees.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeremy Arkes.

Additional information

Responsible Editor: Junsen Zhang

This research was funded by grant R03HD47407 from the National Institute on Child and Health Development.

Appendix

Appendix

1.1 Data description

This part of the Appendix describes how we created the fertility-related behavior/outcome variables.

1.1.1 Sexual activity and contraception variables

For both males and females, we construct variables for sexual activity and (lack of) contraception use based primarily on three questions from the survey: (1) “About how many times have you had sexual intercourse since the last interview?”; (2) “Thinking about all the times that you have had sexual intercourse since the last interview, how many of those times did you or your sexual partner or partners use any method of birth control, including a condom?”; and (3) “About what percentage of time have you and your partner used a condom?” Other lead-in questions were needed to separate nonresponses from no activity. The 2002 and 2003 surveys required an additional variable. An indicator for whether the person used a condom in every sexual encounter superseded the question on how often the person used contraception.

The first two outcomes for whether had any sex and the number of times the person had sex since the last interview (or in the past 12 months for 1997) is based directly from the first question. The number of times the respondent had non-contraceptive sex since the last interview is calculated as the difference between the answers to the number of times the respondent had sex and the number of times the respondent had used birth control. We excluded from the contraception models a few cases in which there were more instances of “used birth control” than “had sex.” The variable for whether had any non-contraceptive sex is constructed from this variable. The variable for the percentage of time the respondent did not use birth control, for those who had any sex, is constructed by dividing the number of times the person had non-contraceptive sex by the number of times the respondent had sex. Sen (2002) uses both of these measures for contraceptive use in an analysis of how alcohol affects sexual behavior. These variables apply for both males and females and are available starting in the first round in 1997. The variable for the percent of time the respondent did not use a condom comes straight from the survey.

Questions about sexual behavior are often sensitive. To address expected nontrivial nonresponse, the NLSY-97 uses follow-up questions. “Refusals” and “I don’t knows” to the numeric question on the number of times the respondent had sex was followed by a bracketing question. While nonresponse to the initial question on the number of times the respondent had sex ranged from 6 to 12%, about 60 to 70% of the initial nonresponders, depending on the year, answered the bracketed question. For the contraception question, the follow-up question asks about the estimated percent of time the respondent used birth control. Exploiting these follow-up questions, we include in our analyses both the numerical answers and the follow-up answers. For the bracketed answers for the level of sexual activity, we use the mid-point of the range. For the highest bracketed category of “201 or more” for the amount of sexual activity a person engages in, we use a value of 250. For non-contraceptive sex, we impute the value for nonresponders based on the number of times they had sex and the bracketed value for the percent of time they used birth control.

1.1.2 Pregnancies, abortions, and births

For pregnancies, abortions, and births, we create different outcomes for males and females because of differences in the type of question asked of males and females and differences in their knowledge of whether a pregnancy or abortion occurred. It is important to identify the timing of the decisions and outcomes because we need to assign these decisions/outcomes to specific periods to match the economic data. Unfortunately, the lack of specificity and the lack of timing questions in some of these variables will cause some inaccuracies in our data, especially for males. The variables used for these outcomes have a surprisingly small percentage of nonresponses—typically less than 1 and 0% in some years for females and between 0 and 3% for males. First, we discuss the outcomes for females.

For pregnancies, our goal is to determine which intervals between interviews the respondent was pregnant. We assign the female respondent as having had a pregnancy begin in the period since the last interview if either: (1) she was pregnant at the time of the interview, (2) she had at least two pregnancies since the last interview, or (3) she had at least one pregnancy since the last interview and was not pregnant at the time of the last interview. With this measure, we may miss some pregnancies that are not known about at the time of an interview. For example, someone may have become pregnant 2 weeks before the 1998 interview and not known about it at the time of the interview. Therefore, in the 1999 interview, they did not have a pregnancy that began since the 1998 interview and may not count the one that started before the previous interview. These data start in the 1998 round.

For abortions, we use questions on the outcomes of each pregnancy the respondent had since the last interview or in the past year. We create a dichotomous indicator for whether the respondent had an abortion since the last interview. This variable is available starting in the 1998 round.

The birth data present a challenge for determining when the decisions are made. Having a child is typically the result of certain decisions on the intensity of preventative measures at the time of conception point and, possibly, a continuous set of decisions throughout the pregnancy to keep the child or abort, and all these decisions could depend on the economy. Thus, the decisions would carry over from one interval (between interviews) to the next interval if a woman were pregnant at the time of the interview.

Our strategy is to focus on the economic conditions at the time of conception. It would be more ideal to consider the time of conception through the first trimester for the period in which the decisions are made. However, we cannot identify the exact date of conception. Instead, we can determine whether the conception occurred since the last interview or before the last interview based on two questions: one on births from pregnancies since the last interview and one on births from previous pregnancies. If a female had a birth from a pregnancy since the last interview, we code her as having given birth. If she had a birth from a previous pregnancy, then we code her as having given birth (or technically, made the decision to give birth) in the previous period—i.e., in the period before the last interview. We use data starting in the 1998 round for this outcome because the 1997 question about pregnancies is not under a specified period.

For males, we use a different set of questions specific to males. For pregnancies, we use a question on whether the male caused a pregnancy since the last interview. Of course, it is possible that some males are not aware that they caused a pregnancy. “Refusals” and “Don’t Knows” account for roughly 1% of all responses—mostly, they were “Refusals.”

Subsequent questions asked about whether any of these pregnancies ended in an abortion and whether any ended in a live birth. From these questions, we create dichotomous indicators for having caused a pregnancy leading to an abortion and having caused a pregnancy leading to a birth. Nonresponses occurred in between 0 and 3% of the answers to these questions. These questions on pregnancies, abortions, and births for males are available starting in the 1999 survey round.

Table 8 List of all explanatory variables used in all models
Table 9 Sample sizes for Tables 3 and 4
Table 10 Sample sizes for Tables 5 and 6
Table 11 Comparison of coefficient estimates on the unemployment rate from models using clustering at the state-year level versus the individual level

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Arkes, J., Klerman, J.A. Understanding the link between the economy and teenage sexual behavior and fertility outcomes. J Popul Econ 22, 517–536 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-007-0172-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00148-007-0172-5

Keywords

JEL Classifications

Navigation