Skip to main content
Log in

The development of the concept of alive by preschoolers through a cognivive conflict teaching intervention

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The aim of this research was the conceptual change and transformation of the intuitive conceptions of preschoolers concerning life through a cognitive—conflict teaching intervention. Young children use the criterion of movement to justify the classification of objects as living or non-living, that is classify plants as non-living because they are immobile and mobile non-alive objects as living. The concept of life includes many sub-concepts and movement is a secondary and not a universal characteristic of living organisms. The subjects of this research were chosen according to their replies concerning the classification of plants as non-living. Subjects of the experimental group received a teaching intervention aiming at the change of their conceptual context about life and focusing mainly at the dependence of an organism to its environment. In the teaching intervention we used the procedure of cognitive conflict through the juxtaposition of live organisms to dead ones and mobile simulations. Our results show that all the children of experimental group presented a conceptual change classifying the plant as alive and using explanations including the dependence on environment and other functions, increasing the number of criteria used for justification of the animal as living.

Résumé

L’objectif de cette recherche est l’étude du changement conceptuel de la notion du vivant chez les enfants d’âge préscolaire. Selon notre hypothèse, il est possible de favoriser le changement conceptuel en soumettant aux enfants des tâches qui incitent à la construction de modèles plus élaborés. Dans le but de mettre cette hypothèse à l’épreuve, nous avons conduit cette recherche avec des élèves dont les connaissances comportaient uniquement des conceptions vitalistes et/ou mécanistes que nous qvons tenté de déstabiliser, afin de les remplacer, à travers des situations didactiques porteuses de conflits cognitifs, par une conception de la notion du vivant plus cohérente, celle de l’échange de l’organisme vivant avec le milieu. Les résultats montrent que les jeunes élèves peuvent comprendre les contradictions lorsque les données qui infirment leurs conceptions initiales sont présentées simultanément. Plus concrètement, lors du post test, les enfants du groupe expérimental se sont montrés capables d’intégrer le monde végétal dans la catégorie des objets vivants, d’abandonner le critère du mouvement lors de la distinction “vivant-non vivant”, ainsi que de produire une argumentation plus fournie pour justifier l’appartenance des animaux à la catégorie du vivant.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Backscheider, A.G., Shatz, M., & Gelman, S. (1993). Preschoolers’ ability to distinguish living kinds as a function of regrowth.Child Development, 64, 1242–1257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baerker, M. (1995). A plant is an animal standing on its head.Journal of Biological Education, 29, 201–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G., & Desforges, C. (1977). Piagenan psychology and education: Time for revision.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 47, 7–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985).Conceptual Development in Childhood. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chevallard, Y. (1985).La transposition didactique. Grenoble: La Pensée Sauvage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., & Mugny, G. (1981).Le developpement social de l’intelligences Paris: Intereditions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enger, E.D., Korrnelink, J.R., Ross, F.C., & Smith R.J. (1994).Concepts in Biology. Wm. C. Brown Publishers.

  • Gelman, R., Spelke, E., & Meck, E. (1983). What preschoolers know about animate and inanimate objects. In D. Rogers & J. Sloboda (Eds.),The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 297–326). New York: Plenum

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S.A., & Kremer, K.E. (1991). Understanding natural cause: Children’s explanations of how objects and their properties originate.Child Development, 62, 396–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, S.S. (1993).Biology Today. Mosby-Year Book, Inc.

  • Hadjinikita, V., Koulaidis, V., & Zogza, V. (1999). Conceptions of pupils of primary school age (5 to 13 years old) and ‘aims-obstacles’ concerning plant nutrition and growth.Pedagogical Review (Greek Journal-in press).

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1997). Qualitative changes in intuitive biology.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 12, 111–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., Siegler, R.S., Richards, D.DS., Inagaki, K., Stavy, R., & Wax, M. (1993). The development of biological knowledge: A multi-national study.Cognitive Development, 8, 47–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (1990). The zone of proximal development as basis for instruction. In L.C. Moll (Ed.),Vygotski and Education (pp. 349–371). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (1994). Young children’s naive theory of biology.Cognition, 50, 171–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1974).The early growth of logic in the child. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jahoda, G. (1958). Child animism: A critical survey of cross-cultural research.Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 197–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurendeau, M., & Pinard, A. (1962).Causal Thinking in the Child: A Genetic and Experimental approach. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Looft, W.R., & Bartz, W.H. (1969). Animism revived.Psychology Bulletin, 71, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Looft, W.R. (1974). Animistic thought in children: Understanding the “living” across its associated attributes.Journal of Genetic Psychology, 124, 235–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, A.M., Linke, R.D., & Sedgwick, P.P. (1979). School children’s criteria for “Alive”: A content analysis approach.The Journal of Psychology, 103, 103–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, A. (1970). Maturations of concepts of life.The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 116, 101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • O-saki, K.M., & Samiroden, W.D. (1990). Children’s conceptions of ‘living’ and ‘dead’.Journal of Biological Education, 24, 199–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perret-Clermont, A.-N. (1979).La construction de l’intelligence dans l’interaction sociale. Berne: Peter Lang

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1929).The Child’s Conception of the World. New York: Harcourt Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravanis, K., & Bagakis, G. (1998). L’education en Sciences Physiques a l’ecole maternelle: Perspective sociocognitive.International Journal of Early Years Education, 6, 315–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravanis, K., & Papamichael, Y. (1995). Procedures didactiques de destabilisation du systeme de representations spontanees des eleves pour la propagation de la lumiere.Didaskalia, 7, 43–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosengren, K.S., Gelman, S.A., Kalish, C.W., & McCormick M. (1991). As time goes by: Children’s understanding of growth in animals.Child Development, 62, 1302–1320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets, P.M. (1973). The animism controversy revisited: A probability analysis.Journal of Genetic Psychology, 123, 219–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stavy, R., & Wax, N. (1989). Children’s conception of plants as living things.Human Development, 32, 88–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starr, C. (1994).Basic Concepts in Biology. International Thomson Publishing.

  • Springer, K., & Keil, F.C. (1991). Early differentiation of causal mechanisms appropriate to biological and nonbiological kinds.Child Development, 62, 767–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tamir, P., Gal-Chopin, R., & Nussinovitz, R. (1981). How do intermediate and junior high school students conceptualize living and non-living.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 18, 241–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L.S. (1986/1934).Thought and Language (A. Kozulin, Ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waheed, T., & Lucas, A.M. (1992). Understanding interrelated topics: Photosynthesis at age 14+Journal of Biological Science, 26, 193–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zogza, V. Sarmonika, M., & Oeconomopoulou, P. (1996).Conceptions of living-non-living and animal-plant: What are the criteria used by preschoolers for their distinction. Paper presented at the 5th Annual Meeting of Greek Psychological Research (Educational psychology) (in Greek).

  • Zogza, V., & Oeconomopoulou, P. (1999). Conceptions of students of 11–15 years of age about plant nutrition.Pedagogical Review (Greek Journal—in press).

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zogza, V., Papamichael, Y. The development of the concept of alive by preschoolers through a cognivive conflict teaching intervention. Eur J Psychol Educ 15, 191–205 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173174

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173174

Key words

Navigation