Advertisement

Canadian Journal of Anesthesia

, Volume 50, Supplement 1, pp R1–R6 | Cite as

Current concepts of consciousness with some implications for anesthesia

  • Bernard J. Baars
Refresher Course Outline Plenary Symposium

Summary and conclusions

The GW theory suggests that consciousness enables multiple networks to cooperate and compete in solving problems, such as retrieval of specific items from immediate memory. The overall function of consciousness is to provide widespread access, which in turn may serve functions of coordination and control. Consciousness is the gateway to the brain, enabling control even of single neurons and whole neuronal populations (Baars, 1988). None of these control functions become directly conscious, of course, but conscious feedback seems required to recruit control by prefrontal networks. In the metaphor of the theatre, it is as if each specialized audience member can decide locally whether or not to be driven by input from the bright spot on stage. Executive functions — the director behind the scenes — is also largely unconscious, often using the actor in the spotlight on the stage of working memory capacity to recruit and trigger specific functions.

Keywords

Left Hemisphere Work Memory Capacity Bright Spot Visual Imagery Binocular Rivalry 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References (presented in alphabetical order)

  1. Baars BJ. Conscious contents provide the nervous system with coherent, global information. In: Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE, Shapiro D (Eds.). Consciousness and Self-Regulation. NY: Plenum Press; 1983: 41.Google Scholar
  2. Baars B. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1988.Google Scholar
  3. Baars B. In the Theater of Consciousness: The workspace of the Mind. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997.Google Scholar
  4. Baars BJ. The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Science 2002; 6: 47–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Damasio AR. Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: a systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition 1989: 33: 25–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. DeHaene S, Naccache L. Toward a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 2001; 79: 1–37.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dennett DC. Are we explaining consciousness yet? Cognition 2001; 79: 221–37.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Edelman GM. Neural Darwinism. New York: Basic Books; 1989.Google Scholar
  9. Edelman GM, Tononi G. A Universe of Consciousness. How Matter Become Imagination. New York: Basic Books; 2000.Google Scholar
  10. John ER. Consciousness & Cognition. 2000: 10: 2.Google Scholar
  11. Kanwisher N. Neural events and perceptual awareness, Cognition 2001; 79: 89–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Llinas R, Ribary U, Contreras D, Pedroarena C. The neuronal basis for consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1998; 353: 1841–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews — Neuroscience 2001; 2: 237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Canadian Anesthesiologists 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The Neurosciences InstituteSan DiegoUSA

Personalised recommendations