Advertisement

“Reinforcement” in behavior theory

  • William N. Schoenfeld
Article
  • 54 Downloads

Abstract

In its Pavlovian context, “reinforcement” was actually a descriptive term for the functional relation between an unconditional and a conditional stimulus. When it was adopted into operant conditioning, “reinforcement” became the central concept and the key operation, but with new qualifications, new referents, and new expectations. Some behavior theorists believed that “reinforcers” comprise a special and limited class of stimuli or events, and they speculated about what the essential “nature of reinforcement” might be. It is now known that any stimulus can serve a reinforcing function, with due recognition of such parameters as subject species characteristics, stimulus intensity, sensory modality, and schedule of application. This paper comments on these developments from the standpoint of reflex behavior theory.

Keywords

Conditional Stimulus Operant Conditioner Conditioning Procedure Stimulus Change Behavior Theory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bersh, P. J.: The influence of two variables upon the establishment of a secondary reinforcer for operant responses. J. Exp. Psychol.41, 62–73, 1951.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bugelski, R. R.: Extinction with and without sub-goal reinforcement. J. Comp. Psychol.26, 121–134, 1938.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Estes, W. K.: An experimental study of punishment. Psychol. Monogr. 57, No. 263, 1944.Google Scholar
  4. Ferster, C. B. and Skinner, B. F.: Schedules of Reinforcement. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Gantt, W. H.: Personal communication.Google Scholar
  6. Glaser, R. (ed.): The Nature of Reinforcement. New York, Academic Press, 1971.Google Scholar
  7. Guthrie, E. R.: Conditioning as a principle of learning. Psychol. Rev.37, 412–428, 1930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Guthrie, E. R.: The Psychology of Learning. New York, Harper, 1935; rev. ed., Harper, New York, 1952.Google Scholar
  9. Hall, J. F.: Classical Conditioning and Instrumental Learning. Philadelphia, Lippincott, 1976.Google Scholar
  10. Hilgard, E. R.: Theories of Learning, 2nd. ed. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1956; 3rd ed., with G. H. Bower, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1966.Google Scholar
  11. Hull, C. L.: Principles of Behavior. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.Google Scholar
  12. Keller, F. S. and Schoenfeld, W. N.: Principles of Psychology. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1950.Google Scholar
  13. Konorski, J. and Miller, S.: On two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol.16, 264–272, 1937 (a).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Konorski, J. and Miller, S.: Further remarks on two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol.17, 405–407, 1937 (b).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Libby, A.: Two variables in the acquisition of depressant properties by a stimulus. J. Exp. Psychol.42, 100–107, 1951.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Miller, N. E.: Learnable drives and rewards.In Stevens, S. S. (ed.) Handbook of Experimental Psychology, New York, Wiley, 1951.Google Scholar
  17. Mueller, C. G., Jr. and Schoenfeld, W. N.: Edwin R. Guthrie.In Estes W. K.et al. Modern Learning Theory, New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1954.Google Scholar
  18. Reese, E. P.: Human Behavior; Analysis and Application, 2nd. ed. Dubuque, Wm. C. Brown, 1978.Google Scholar
  19. Schoenfeld, W. N.: Some old work for modern behavior theory. Cond. Reflex,1, 219–223, 1966.Google Scholar
  20. Schoenfeld, W. N.: Problems of modern behavior theory. Cond. Reflex7, 33–65, 1972.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Schoenfeld, W. N.: The “response” in behaviortheory. Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci.11, 129–149, 1976.Google Scholar
  22. Schoenfeld, W. N., Antonitis, J. J. and Bersh, P. J.: A preliminary study of training conditions necessary for secondary reinforcement. J. Exp. Psychol.40, 40–45, 1950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Schoenfeld, W. N. and Cole, B. K.: Stimulus Schedules: The t-t Systems. New York, Harper and Row, 1972.Google Scholar
  24. Schoenfeld, W. N. and Cole, B. K.: What is a “schedule of reinforcement”? Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci.10, 52–61, 1975.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Schoenfeld, W. N., Cole, B. K., Lang, J. and Mankoff, R.: “Contingency” in behavior theory.In McGuigan F. J. and Lumsden, D. B. (eds.) Contemporary Approaches to Conditioning and Learning, V. H. Winston, Washington, D. C., 1973.Google Scholar
  26. Skinner, B. F.: The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. J. Gen. Psychol.5, 427–458, 1931. See also the added foreword to this paper when reprinted in B. F. Skinner, Cumulative Record, New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1959, pp. 319–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Skinner, B. F.: Two types of conditioned reflex: a reply to Konorski and Miller, J. Gen. Psychol.16, 272–279, 1937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Skinner, B. F.: The Behavior of Organisms. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1938.Google Scholar
  29. Skinner, B. F.: Contingencies of reinforcement. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.Google Scholar
  30. Warden, C. J.et al.: Animal Motivation. New York, Columbia University Press, 1931.Google Scholar
  31. Wike, E. L.: Secondary Reinforcement. New York, Harper and Row, 1966.Google Scholar
  32. Zeaman, D.: Response latency as a function of amount of reinforcement. J. Exp. Psychol.39, 466–483, 1949.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Zeaman, D. and House, B. J.: Response latency at zero drive after varying numbers of reinforcements, J Exp. Psychol.40, 570–583, 1950.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© J. B. Lippincott Company 1978

Authors and Affiliations

  • William N. Schoenfeld
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.|Department of PsychologyQueens College (C.U.N.Y.)Flushing
  2. 2.Cornell University Medical CollegeUSA

Personalised recommendations