Skip to main content
Log in

How and why do nectar-foraging bumblebees initiate movements between inflorescences of wild bergamot Monarda fistulosa (Lamiaceae)?

  • Original Papers
  • Published:
Oecologia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

By experimental manipulation of the nectar in flowers, I characterized the decision-making process used by nectar-gathering bumblebees for initiating movements between inflorescences of wild bergamot. The decision-making process has these characteristics: departure from an inflorescence is less likely as nectar rewards increase; departure decisions are based on the amount of nectar in the last flower probed and are not influenced by the nectar rewards in either the previously probed flower or the previously visited inflorescence; the number of flowers already probed at an inflorescence influences departure decisions weakly; a bees' response (to stay or to depart) to a given size of nectar reward is variable. Since previously proposed foraging rules do not accord with this description, I propose a new rule. I show by experiment that the movements made by bumblebees enhance foraging success.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bovet P (1985) The adaptive fitness of randomness in choice and foraging behavior. Beh Brain Sci 8:315–330

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron SA (1981) Chemical signals in bumblebee foraging. Beh Ecol Sociobiol 9:257–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnov EL (1976) Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theor Pop Biol 9:129–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheverton J (1982) Bumblebees may use a suboptimal arbitrary handedness to solve complex foraging decisions. Anim Beh 30:934–935

    Google Scholar 

  • Cibula DA, Zimmerman M (1986) The effect of plant density on departure decisions: testing the marginal value theorem using bumblebees and Delphinium nelsonii. Oikos 43:154–158

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowie RJ (1977) Optimal foraging in great tits (Parus major). Nature 268:137–138

    Google Scholar 

  • Cresswell JE (1989) Optimal foraging theory applied to bumblebees gathering nectar from wild bergamot. Ph.D. thesis, University of Michigan, MI, USA

  • Cruden RW, Hermanutz L, Shuttleworth J (1984) The pollination biology and breeding system of Monarda fistulosa (Labiatae). Oecologia 64:104–110

    Google Scholar 

  • Emlen JM (1966) The role of time and energy in food preference. Am Nat 100:611–617

    Google Scholar 

  • Galen C, Plowright RC (1985) The effects of nectar level and flower development on pollen carry-over in inflorescences of fireweed (Epilobium angustifolium) (Onagraceae). Can J Bot 63:488–491

    Google Scholar 

  • Green RF (1980) Bayesian birds: a simple example of Oaten's stochastic model of optimal foraging. Theor Pop Biol 18:244–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartling LK, Plowright RC (1978) Foraging by bumble bees on patches of artificial flowers: a laboratory study. Can J Zool 57:1866–1870

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges CM (1981) Optimal foraging in bumblebees: hunting by expectation. Beh Ecol Sociobiol 9:41–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges CM (1985a) Bumble bee foraging: the threshold departure rule. Ecology 66:179–187

    Google Scholar 

  • Hodges CM (1985b) Bumble bee foraging: energetic consequences of using a threshold departure rule. Ecology 66:188–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwasa I, Higashi M, Yamamura N (1981) Prey distribution as a factor determining the choice of optimal foraging strategy. Am Nat 117:710–723

    Google Scholar 

  • Janetos AC, Cole BJ (1981) Imperfectly optimal animals. Beh Ecol Sociobiol 9:203–209

    Google Scholar 

  • Kato M (1988) Bumblebee visits to Impatiens spp: pattern and efficiency. Oecologia 76:364–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Ryan JC, Charnov EL (1974) Hunting by expectation or optimal foraging? A study of patch use by chickadees. Anim Behav 22:953–964

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1984) Downy woodpecker foraging behavior: efficient sampling in simple stochastic environments. Ecology 65:166–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1985) Sampling behavior of starlings foraging in simple patchy environments. Beh Ecol Sociobiol 16:135–142

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur RH, Pianka ER (1966) On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am Nat 100:603–609

    Google Scholar 

  • Marden JH (1984) Remote perception of floral nectar by bumblebees. Oecologia 64:232–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard-Smith J (1978) Optimization theory in evolution. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 9:31–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazur JE (1983) Reply to staddon and Hinson. Science 221:977

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenna MA, Thomson JD (1988) A technique for sampling and measuring small amounts of floral nectar. Ecology 69:1306–1307

    Google Scholar 

  • McNair JN (1985) Optimal foraging for operant conditioners. Beh Brain Sci 8:343–344

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara J, Houston A (1980) The application of statistical decision theory to animal behavior. J Theor Biol 85:673–690

    Google Scholar 

  • Ollason JG (1980) Learning to forage- optimally? Theor Pop Biol 18:44–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Pleasants JD (1983) Nectar production patterns in Ipomopsis aggregata (Polemoniaceae). Am J Bot 70:1468–1475

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyke GH (1979) Optimal foraging in bumblebees: rule of departure between flowers within inflorescences. Anim Beh 27:1167–1181

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyke GH (1982) Foraging in bumblebees: rule of departure from an inflorescence. Can J Zool 60:417–428

    Google Scholar 

  • Real L, Caraco T (1986) Risk and foraging in stochastic environments. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 17:371–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Real L, Rathcke BJ (1988) Patterns of individual variability in floral resources. Ecology 69:728–735

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoener TW (1971) Theory of feeding strategies. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 2:369–404

    Google Scholar 

  • sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1981) Biometry (2nd ed.) W.H. Freeman and Co., NY, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon JER, Hinson JM (1983) Optimization: a result or a mechanism? Science 221:976–977

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens DW, Krebs JR (1986) Foraging Theory. Princeton University Press, NJ, USA

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson JD, Maddison WP, Plowright RC (1982) Behavior of bumbleebee pollinators of Aralia hispida Vent. (Araliaceae). Oecologia 54:326–336

    Google Scholar 

  • Waddington KD (1981) Factors influencing pollen flow in bumblebee-pollinated Delphiniun virescens. Oikos 37:153–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman M (1983) Plant reproduction and optimal foraging: experimental nectar manipulations in Delphinium nelsonii. Oikos 41:57–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman M, Pyke GH (1986) Reproduction in Polemonium: patterns and implications of floral nectar production and standing crops. Am J Bot 73:1405–1415

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cresswell, J.E. How and why do nectar-foraging bumblebees initiate movements between inflorescences of wild bergamot Monarda fistulosa (Lamiaceae)?. Oecologia 82, 450–460 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00319785

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00319785

Key words

Navigation