Advertisement

Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science

, Volume 2, Issue 1–2, pp 59–69 | Cite as

Variations in relationship between fundamental frequency and intensity in speech of children during development

  • Sumanlata GautamEmail author
  • Latika Singh
Research Paper
  • 81 Downloads

Abstract

Prosody development provides insight into the development of a highly specialized system that is unique to humans. Prosody is mainly encoded by regulating the fundamental frequency, intensity and duration of various tokens during discourse construction. Interplay between the fundamental frequency and the intensity plays a crucial role in formulating prosody and adult speakers modulate these parameters to produce rhythmic sentences. However, it is not known, how this relationship between these two parameters evolve with age in the children. The present study tries to fill this gap partially by investigating the statistical nature of the correlation between fundamental frequency and intensity in the speech of 42 children and 25 adults between the ages of 4–25 years. Statistically significant differences were found in the distributions of correlations between children and adults. The difference can be attributed to evolving skills of children in encoding prosodic cues through modulation of intensity by alterations in flow of air from the lungs and controlling the vibrations of vocal folds.

Keywords

Prosody development Correlation between fundamental frequency and intensity 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Prof. Prabha Sharma for her precious critique and proof reading of the manuscript. We also wish to thank all the participants and the parents of the children who participated in this study.

Author contributions

Both the authors drafted the manuscript and contributed in concept, design, acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data. Both the authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

We have read and understood policy on declaration of interests and declare that we have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Ahissar, E., Nagarajan, S., Ahissar, M., Protopapas, A., Mahncke, H., & Merzenich, M. M. (2001). Speech comprehension is correlated with temporal response patterns recorded from auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(23), 13367–13372.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.201400998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Assmann, P. F., Nearey, T. M. & S. V. B. (2013). Developmental patterns in children’s speech: patterns of spectral change in vowels. In Vowel inherent spectral change. (pp. 199–230). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg.Google Scholar
  3. Ballard, K. J., Djaja, D., Arciuli, J., James, D. G. H., & van Doorn, J. (2012). Developmental trajectory for production of prosody: lexical stress contrastivity in children ages 3 to 7 years and in adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(6), 1822–1835.  https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0257).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Boersma, P. (1993). Accurate short-term analysis of the fundamental frequency and the harmonics-to-noise ratio of a sampled sound. Proceedings of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences, 17, 97–110.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5(9/10), 341–347.  https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e31821473f7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bonneh, Y. S., Levanon, Y., Dean-Pardo, O., Lossos, L., & Adini, Y. (2011). Abnormal speech spectrum and increased pitch variability in young autistic children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00237.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Campbell, N., & Beckman, M. E. (1997). Stress, prominence, and spectral tilt. Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications, (September), 67–70.Google Scholar
  8. Clark, E. V., Gelman, S. A., & Lane, N. M. (1985). Compound nouns and category structure in young children. Child Development, 56(1), 84–94.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Crystal, D. (1986). Prosodic development. Language acquisition.Google Scholar
  10. Dore, J., Franklin, M. B., Miller, R. T., & Ramer, A. L. H. (1976). Transitional phenomena in early language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 13–28.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900001288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eguchi, S., & Hirsh, I. J. (1968). Development of speech sounds in children. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 257, 1–51.Google Scholar
  12. Féry, C., Pandey, P., & Kentner, G. (2016). The prosody of focus and givenness in Hindi and Indian English. Studies in Language, 40(2), 302–339.  https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.40.2.02fer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gautam S. (2017). Development of spectro-temporal features of speech (Doctoral dissertation), Dec 2017, The NorthCap University, Gurugram.Google Scholar
  14. Gautam, S., & Singh, L. (2017). Development of spectro-temporal features of speech in children. International Journal of Speech Technology, 20(3), 543–551.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10772-017-9424-2.
  15. Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(16), 10911–10916.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.122368599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Greenberg, S. (1999). Speaking in shorthand—a syllable-centric perspective for understanding pronunciation variation. Speech Communication, 29(2), 159–176.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(99)00050-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Greenberg, S. (2005). A multi-tier framework for understanding spoken language. Listening to Speech: An Auditory Perspective. Google Scholar
  18. Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. Explorations in language study.  https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-443701-2.50025-1.
  19. Hillenbrand, J. M., Getty, L. A., Clark, M. J., & Wheeler, K. (1995). Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(5), 3099–3111.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.411872.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Hosom, J.-P., Shriberg, L., & Green, J. R. (2004). Diagnostic assessment of childhood apraxia of speech using automatic speech recognition (ASR) methods. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 12(4), 167–171.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biotechadv.2011.08.021.Secreted.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Huber, J. E., Stathopoulos, E. T., Curione, G. M., Ash, T. A., & Johnson, K. (1999). Formants of children, women, and men: The effects of vocal intensity variation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106(3), 1532–1542.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.427150.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Kaplan, E. (1970). Inntonation and language acquistion. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 1–21.Google Scholar
  23. Kent, R. D. (1976). Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic studies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 19(3), 421–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kent, R. D., & Forner, L. L. (1980). Speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 157–168.Google Scholar
  25. Kent, R. D., Read, C., & Kent, R. D. (1992). The acoustic analysis of speech. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.Google Scholar
  26. Koike, K. J., & Asp, C. W. (1981). Tennessee test of rhythm and intonation patterns. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 46(1), 81–7.Google Scholar
  27. Lee, S., Potamianos, A., & Narayanan, S. (1999). Acoustics of children’s speech: developmental changes of temporal and spectral parameters. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(3), 1455–1468.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.426686.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Loeb, D. F., & Allen, G. D. (1993). Preschoolers’ imitation of intonation contours. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(1), 4–13.  https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3601.04.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Merewether, F. C., & Alpert, M. (1990). The components and neuroanatomic bases of prosody. Journal of Communication Disorders, 23(4–5), 325–336.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0021-9924(90)90007-L.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Oller, D. K. (1980). The emergence of the sounds of speech in infancy. Child Phonology, Vol. 1, 93–112.  https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-770601-6.50011-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Panagos, J. M., & Prelock, P. A. (1997). Prosodic analysis of child speech. Topics in Language Disorders, 17(4), 1. http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=jlh&AN=1998031652&site=ehost-live%5CnPublisher; http://www.cinahl.com/cgi-bin/refsvc?jid=1242&accno=1998031652.
  32. Pandey, P. (2016). Indian English prosody. In Communicating with Asia: The Future of English as a Global Language (pp. 56–68).  https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107477186.005.
  33. Rabiner, L. R. (1977). On the use of autocorrelation analysis for pitch detection. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing, 25(1), 24–33.  https://doi.org/10.1109/TASSP.1977.1162905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rosen, S. (1992). Temporal information in speech: acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1992.0070.
  35. Sharda, M., Subhadra, T. P., Sahay, S., Nagaraja, C., Singh, L., Mishra, R., et al. (2010). Sounds of melody-Pitch patterns of speech in autism. Neuroscience Letters, 478(1), 42–45.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.066.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Shriberg, L. D., Campbell, T. F., Karlsson, H. B., Brown, R. L., Mcsweeny, J. L., & Nadler, C. J. (2003). A diagnostic marker for childhood apraxia of speech: the lexical stress ratio. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 17(7), 549–574.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920031000138123.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Singh, L., & Singh, N. C. (2008). The development of articulatory signatures in children. Developmental Science, 11(4), 467–473.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00692.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Smith, B. L. (1992). Relationships between duration and temporal variability in childrens speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(4), 2165–2174.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.403675.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Snow, D. (1994). Phrase-final syllable lengthening and intonation in early child speech. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 37(4), 831.  https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3704.831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Stoel-Gammon, C. (1992). Prelinguistic vocal development: Measurements and Predictions. Phonological Developments: Models, Research, Implications, 439–456.Google Scholar
  41. Traunmüller, H., & Eriksson, A. (2000). Acoustic effects of variation in vocal effort by men, women, and children. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 107, 3438–3451.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.429414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zhang, Y., Nissen, S. L., & Francis, A. L. (2008). Acoustic characteristics of English lexical stress produced by native Mandarin speakers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(6), 4498–4513.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2902165.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Computer Science and EngineeringThe NorthCap UniversityGurugramIndia
  2. 2.Department of Computer Science and EngineeringAnsal UniversityGurugramIndia

Personalised recommendations