Non-action Video Game Training Ameliorates Cognitive Decline Associated with Sleep Disturbance
- 109 Downloads
Abstract
Purpose
Sleep disturbance is quite prevalent among students which leads to deleterious consequences on cognitive behavior. Non-action video game training has been shown to improve many aspects of higher order mental functions; therefore, we investigated the effect of same on sleep quality and cognitive functions in sleep-disturbed university students.
Methods
The study was a randomized controlled type where participants (n = 30) were assigned into two groups: control and experimental. The subjects in the experimental group completed 4 weeks of computerized non-action video game training. Pre- and post-training measures were taken for sleep quality using Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) and Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS). Cognitive functions were tested using PennCNP, computerized neuropsychological battery. Electrophysiological correlates of cognition were evaluated by P300 event-related potential using auditory oddball paradigm.
Result
We found significant decrease in both PSQI and ESS scores in the experimental group as compared to the control group, indicating improvement in sleep. The scores for reaction time, visual memory, and logical reasoning showed improvement in video game learners as compared to the control group. The P300 data showed a decrease in latency and increase in amplitude with video game training in sleep-disturbed students indicating improvement in cognition. Correlation analysis demonstrated that the increase in cognitive function was associated with gains in sleep quality.
Conclusion
The present study showed that 4 weeks of non-action video game training improved sleep quality and cognitive functions in sleep-disturbed university students.
Keywords
Video game Sleep Cognition P300 PSQINotes
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge the support of University of Pennsylvania, for giving us the access to use their cognitive test battery, PennCNP. We thank D. J. Buysse and M. W. Johns for giving us the permission to use their copyright questionnaires. Finally, we thank the Neurophysiology Lab, Centre for Physiotherapy and Rehabilitation Sciences (CPRS), Jamia Millia Islamia (JMI), India for their ongoing hard work and support.
Author contributions
AA takes full responsibility for the integrity of the work as a whole, from inception to finished article.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of interest
No competing financial interests exist.
References
- 1.Manzar MD, Zannat W, Kaur M, Hussain ME. Sleep in university students across years of university education and gender influences. Int J Adolesc Med Health. 2015;27(3):341–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 2.Sweileh WM, Ali IA, Sawalha AF, Abu-Taha AS, Sa’ed HZ, Al-Jabi SW. Sleep habits and sleep problems among Palestinian students. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Mental Health. 2011;5(1):25.Google Scholar
- 3.Gaultney JF. The prevalence of sleep disorders in college students: impact on academic performance. J Am Coll Health. 2010;59(2):91–7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 4.Harrison Y, Horne JA, Rothwell A. Prefrontal neuropsychological effects of sleep deprivation in young adults—a model for healthy aging? Sleep. 2000;23(8):1067–73.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 5.Babkoff H, Zukerman GI, Fostick L, Ben-Artzi EL. Effect of the diurnal rhythm and 24 h of sleep deprivation on dichotic temporal order judgment. J Sleep Res. 2005;14(1):7–15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 6.Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour: implications for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162(3):413–9.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 7.Park DC. The basic mechanisms accounting for age-related decline in cognitive function. In: Cognitive aging: a primer, vol. 11(1);2000. p.3–19.Google Scholar
- 8.Haimov I, Shatil E. Cognitive training improves sleep quality and cognitive function among older adults with insomnia. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4):61390.Google Scholar
- 9.Lee HJ, Kim L, Kim YK, Suh KY, Han J, Park MK, Park KW, Lee DH. Auditory event-related potentials and psychological changes during sleep deprivation. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;50(1):1–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 10.Twamley EW, Jeste DV, Bellack AS. A review of cognitive training in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(2):359–82.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 11.Green CS, Bavelier D. The cognitive neuroscience of video games. In: Digital media: transformations in human communication. 2006. p. 211–23.Google Scholar
- 12.Ferguson CJ. The good, the bad and the ugly: a meta-analytic review of positive and negative effects of violent video games. Psychiatr Q. 2007;78(4):309–16.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 13.Boot WR, Blakely DP, Simons DJ. Do action video games improve perception and cognition? Front Psychol. 2011;2:226.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 14.Dye MW, Green CS, Bavelier D. Increasing speed of processing with action video games. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(6):321–6.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 15.Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji F, Janowich J, Kong E, Larraburo Y, Rolle C, Johnston E, Gazzaley A. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature. 2013;501(7465):97.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 16.Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 17.Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14(6):540–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 18.Gur RC, Richard J, Hughett P, Calkins ME, Macy L, Bilker WB, Brensinger C, Gur RE. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct validation. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;187(2):254–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 19.Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Brereton AV, Tonge BJ. Movement preparation in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: a serial choice reaction time task involving motor reprogramming. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):79–88.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 20.Magnussen S. Implicit visual working memory. Scand J Psychol. 2009;50(6):535–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 21.Goel V, Makale M, Grafman J. The hippocampal system mediates logical reasoning about familiar spatial environments. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(4):654–64.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 22.Hull J, Harsh J. P300 and sleep-related positive waveforms (P220, P450, and P900) have different determinants. J Sleep Res. 2001;10(1):9–17.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 23.Chatterjee A, Ray K, Panjwani U, Thakur L, Anand JP. Meditation as an intervention for cognitive disturbances following total sleep deprivation. Indian J Med Res. 2012;136(6):1031.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 24.Sternberg DA, Ballard K, Hardy JL, Katz B, Doraiswamy PM, Scanlon M. The largest human cognitive performance dataset reveals insights into the effects of lifestyle factors and aging. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;20(7):292.Google Scholar
- 25.Lee HJ, Kim L, Suh KY. Cognitive deterioration and changes of P300 during total sleep deprivation. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2003;57(5):490–6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 26.Fogel SM, Smith CT. Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and stage 2 sleep. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(3):250–5.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 27.Vertes RP. Memory consolidation in sleep: dream or reality. Neuron. 2004;44(1):135–48.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 28.Carney CE, Waters WF. Effects of a structured problem-solving procedure on pre-sleep cognitive arousal in college students with insomnia. Behav Sleep Med. 2006;4(1):13–28.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 29.Gais S, Born J. Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2004;101(7):2140–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 30.Raouafi S, Etindele Sosso FA. Cyberpsychology: video games as a perspective for cognitive training. Ment Health Addict Res. 2017;2(3):1–2.Google Scholar
- 31.Ballesteros S, Prieto A, Mayas J, Toril P, Pita C, Ponce de León L, Reales JM, Waterworth J. Brain training with non-action video games enhances aspects of cognition in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;14(6):277.Google Scholar
- 32.Basak C, Boot WR, Voss MW, Kramer AF. Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuate cognitive decline in older adults? Psychol Aging. 2008;23(4):765.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- 33.Boot WR, Kramer AF, Simons DJ, Fabiani M, Gratton G. The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2008;129(3):387–98.Google Scholar
- 34.Etindele Sosso FA. Visual dot interaction with short-term memory. Neurodegener Dis Manag. 2017;7(3):183–90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 35.Etindele Sosso FA, Hito M, Bern S. Basic activity of neurons in the dark during somnolence induced by anesthesia. J Neurol Neurosci. 2017. https://doi.org/10.21767/2171-6625.1000203.Google Scholar
- 36.Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(6):781–91.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 37.Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23(10):475–83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 38.Mishra J, Zinni M, Bavelier D, Hillyard SA. Neural basis of superior performance of action videogame players in an attention-demanding task. J Neurosci. 2011;31(3):992–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- 39.Maillot P, Perrot A, Hartley A. Effects of interactive physical-activity video-game training on physical and cognitive function in older adults. Psychol Aging. 2012;27(3):589.PubMedGoogle Scholar