No Evidence for Performance Improvements in Episodic Memory Due to Fidgeting, Doodling or a “Neuro-Enhancing” Drink
Abstract
The media advertises fidgeting devices and nutrition supplements as possible ways to enhance cognition, which often have not been validated. The current study investigates the influence of fidget spinners, stress balls, doodling, and a supposedly neuro-enhancing beverage called “Neuronade” on the encoding performance in an episodic memory task using a within-subject design. Sport students (n = 58, Mage = 24 years; 24 women) learned the method of loci as an episodic memory strategy. Memory encoding took place under different conditions, administered in a partially counterbalanced order. Compared with the baseline, none of the products resulted in memory enhancements, with the fidget spinner and doodling even reducing performances, and the stress ball and the Neuronade not changing cognitive performance. These findings strongly suggest that the scientific community should become more active in investigating claims of supposedly neuro-enhancing products.
Keywords
Episodic memory Motor activity Food supplements Cognitive enhancementNotes
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Daniel Bill, Keito Philippi, Janine Vieweg, and Manuel Zachej for their help with the data entry and for helpful discussions. The authors would also like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments.
Author Contributions
The authors both contributed equally to this study. S. Schaefer developed the study concept. Both authors contributed to the study design and cooperated in conducting the literature review. S. Schaefer collected the data. G. Amico analyzed and interpreted the data, with input from S. Schaefer. G. Amico led the drafting of the manuscript, with substantial contributions from S. Schaefer.
Funding
This work was supported by the Saarland University.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- Andrade, J. (2010). What does doodling do? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 100–106. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22, 366–377. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Benson, S., Downey, L. A., Stough, C., Wetherell, M., Zangara, A., & Scholey, A. (2014). An acute, double-blind, placebo-controlled cross-over study of 320 mg and 640 mg doses of Bacopa monnieri (CDRI 08) on multitasking stress reactivity and mood. Phytotherapy Research, 28, 551–559. https://doi.org/10.1002/ptr.5029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Biel, L. (2017). Fidget toys or focus tools? Autism File, 74, 12–13. Retrieved from https://www.sensorysmarts.com/AADJun17.pdf. Accessed 5 June 2018.Google Scholar
- Boggs, J. B., Cohen, J. L., & Marchand, G. C. (2017). The effects of doodling on recall ability. Psychological Thought, 10, 206–216. https://doi.org/10.5964/psyct.v10i1.217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brehmer, Y., Stoll, G., Bergner, S., Benoit, R., Oertzen, T., & Lindenberger U. (2005). Selection of high-imagery words for the study of episodic memory. Technical Report, Saarland University. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11780/211. Accessed 5 June 2018.
- Bundesverband des Spielwaren-Einzelhandels (2017). Fidget spinner: Umsatz knackt Millionen-Euro-Grenze. In Pressemitteilung “Deutsche im Handkreiselfieber”. Retrieved from https://www.bvspielwaren.de/PDF/2017-05-24_PM_BVS-FidgetSpinner.pdf. Accessed 5 June 2018.
- Calabrese, C., Gregory, W. L., Leo, M., Kraemer, D., Bone, K., & Oken, B. (2008). Effects of a standardized Bacopa monnieri extract on cognitive performance, anxiety, and depression in the elderly: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14, 707–713. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2008.0018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Chan, E. (2012). The negative effect of doodling on visual recall task performance. UBCUJP,1, 59-64. Retrieved from http://ojs.library.ubc.ca/index.php/ubcujp/article/view/2526. Accessed 25 June 2018.
- Chinchalkar, S. J., & Pipicelli, J. G. (2009). Addressing extensor Digitorum Communis adherence after metacarpal fracture with the use of a circumferential fracture brace. Journal of Hand Therapy, 22, 377–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jht.2009.06.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, E. J., Bravi, R., & Minciacchi, D. (2018). The effect of fidget spinners on fine motor control. Nature, 8, 3144. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21529-0.Google Scholar
- Day, L. (2010). Building blocks of ADHD. Early Years Educator, 12, 36–38. https://doi.org/10.12968/eyed.2010.12.6.78595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Downey, L. A., Kean, J., Nemeh, F., Lau, A., Poll, A., Gregory, R., & Stough, C. (2013). An acute, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study of 320 mg and 640 mg doses of a special extract of Bacopa monnieri (CDRI 08) on sustained cognitive performance. Phytotherapy Research, 27, 1407–1413. https://doi.org/10.1002/ptr.4864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Farley, J., Risko, E. F., & Kingstone, A. (2013). Everyday attention and lecture retention: The effects of time, fidgeting, and mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Galton, F. (1885). The measure of fidget. Nature, 32, 174–175. https://doi.org/10.1038/032174b0.Google Scholar
- Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: on land and underwater. British Journal of Psychology, 66, 325–331. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01468.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Grant, H. M., Bredhal, L. C., Clay, J., Ferrie, J., Groves, J. E., Mc Dorman, T. A., & Dark, V. J. (1998). Context-dependent memory for meaningful material: information for students. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 617–623. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(1998120)12:6<617::AID-ACP542>3.0.CO;2-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Graziano, P. A., Garcia, A. M., & Landis, D. L. (2018). To fidget or not to fidget, that is the question: a systematic classroom evaluation of fidget spinners among young children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054718770009.
- Greely, H., Sahakian, B., Harris, J., Kessler, R. C., Gazzaniga, M., Campbell, P., & Farah, M. J. (2008). Towards responsible use of cognitive enhancing drugs by the healthy. Nature, 456, 702–705. https://doi.org/10.1038/456702a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gupta, S. (2016). Doodling: the artistry of the roving metaphysical mind. Journal of Mental Health and Human Behaviour, 21, 16–19. https://doi.org/10.4103/0971-8990.182097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Guttentag, R. E. (1989). Age differences in dual-task performance: procedures, assumptions, and results. Developmental Review, 9, 146–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(89)90027-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hartanto, T. A., Krafft, C. E., Losif, A. M., & Schweitzer, J. B. (2016). A trial by trial analysis reveals more intense physical activity is associated with better cognitive control performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 618–626. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1044511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hudson, B. F., Ogden, J., & Whiteley, M. S. (2015). Randomized controlled trial to compare the effect of simple distraction interventions on pain and anxiety experienced during conscious surgery. European Journal of Pain, 19, 1447–1455. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hung, S. K., Perry, R., & Ernst, E. (2011). The effectiveness and efficacy of Rhodiola rosea L.: a systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Phytomedicine, 18, 235–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.phymed.2010.08.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ishaque, S., Shamseer, L., Bukutu, C., & Vohra, S. (2012). Rhodiola rosea for physical and mental fatigue: a systematic review. BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 12, 70. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6882-12-70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
- Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25, 2027–2037. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614548725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kelly, M. E., Loughrey, D., Lawlor, B. A., Robertson, I. H., Walsh, C., & Brennan, S. (2014). The impact of cognitive training and mental stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aging Research Reviews, 15, 28–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2014.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kennedy, D. O., Scholey, A. B., & Wesnes, K. A. (2000). The dose-dependent cognitive effects of acute administration of Ginkgo biloba to healthy young volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 151, 416–423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. (1990). On the locus and process of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 26, 894–904. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing: Age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12, 230–237. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Aging, 15, 417–436. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.15.3.417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Maher, B. (2008). Poll results: look who’s doping. Nature, 452, 674–675. https://doi.org/10.1038/452674a.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Stanford Center on Longevity. (2014). A consensus on the brain training industry from the scientific community. http://longevity.stanford.edu/a-consensus-on-the-brain-training-industry-from-the-scientific-community-2/. Accessed 22 June 2018.
- Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? a meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nathan, P. J., Clarke, J., Lloyd, J., Hutchison, C. W., Downey, L., & Stough, C. (2001). The acute effects of an extract of Bacopa monniera (Brahmi) on cognitive function in healthy normal subjects. Human Psychopharmacology, 16, 345–351. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nathan, D. M., Tanner, S., Lloyd, J., Harrison, B., Curran, L., Oliver, C., & Stough, C. (2004). Effects of a combined extract of Ginkgo biloba and Bacopa monniera on cognitive function in healthy humans. Human Psychopharmacology, 19, 91–96. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.86.3.214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., & Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465, 775–779. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pase, M. P., Kean, J., Sarris, J., Neale, C., Scholey, A. B., & Stough, C. (2012). The cognitive-enhancing effects of Bacopa monnieri: a systematic review of randomized, controlled human clinical trials. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 18, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2011.0367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Plewman, R. (2018). In it to spin it: fidget spinners now account for 17 percent of all daily toy sales. [webpage]. Retrieved from http://intelligence.slice.com/blog/2017/spin-fidget-spinners-now-account-17-percent-daily-toy-sales. Accessed 18 June 2018.
- Propper, R. E., McGraw, S. E., Brunyé, T. T., & Weiss, M. (2013). Getting a grip on memory: Unilateral hand clenching alters episodic recall. PLoS One, 8, e62474. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Roodenrys, S., Booth, D., Bulzomi, S., Phipps, A., Micallef, C., & Smoker, J. (2002). Chronic effects of Brahmi (Bacopa monnieri) on human memory. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 279–281. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00419-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sarver, D. E., Rapport, M. D., Kofler, M. J., Raiker, J. S., & Friedman, L. M. (2015). Hyperactivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Impairing deficit or compensatory behavior? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1219–1232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0011-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schaefer, S. (2014). The ecological approach to cognitive-motor dual-tasking: findings on the effects of expertise and age. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01167.Google Scholar
- Schaefer, S., Krampe, R. T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2008). Age differences between children and young adults in the dynamics of dual-task prioritization: Body (balance) versus mind (memory). Developmental Psychology, 44, 747–757. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schecter, R. A., Shah, J., Fruitman, K., & Milanaik, R. L. (2017). Fidget spinners: purported benefits, adverse effects and accepted alternatives. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 29, 616–618. https://doi.org/10.1097/MOP.0000000000000523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the COGITO study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2010.00027.Google Scholar
- Shevstov, V. A., Zholus, B. I., Shervarly, V. I., Vol'skij, V. B., Korovin, Y. P., Khristich, M. P., & Wikman, G. (2003). A randomized trial of two different doses of a SHR-5 Rhodiola rosea extract versus placebo and control of capacity for mental work. Phytomedicine, 10, 95–105. https://doi.org/10.1078/094471103321659780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Solomon, P. R., Adams, F., Silver, A., Zimmer, J., & De Veaux, R. (2002). Ginkgo for memory enhancement: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 288, 835–840. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.288.7.835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stalvey, S., & Brasell, H. (2006). Using stress balls to focus the attention of sixth-grade learners. Journal of At-Risk Issues, 12, 7–16.Google Scholar
- Steenbergen, L., Sellaro, R., Hommel, B., Lindenberger, U., Kühn, S., & Colzato, L. (2015). “Unfocus” on foc.us: Commercial tDCS headset impairs working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 234, 637–643. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4391-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Steer, J., & Horstmann, K. (2009). Helping kids and teens with ADHD in school. London and Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.Google Scholar
- Volke, C. (2018).12er Pack Neuronade® Think Drink [Webpage]. Retrieved from https://neuronade.com/shop/neuronade-12er-pack/. Accessed 5 June 2018.
- Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler adult intelligence scale - revised (WAIS-R). New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
- Weinmann, S., Roll, S., Schwarzbach, C., Vauth, C., & Willich, S. (2010). Effects of Ginkgo biloba in dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatrics, 10, 14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-10-14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar