Advertisement

Journal of Cognitive Enhancement

, Volume 3, Issue 1, pp 30–51 | Cite as

Cognitive Training for Military Application: a Review of the Literature and Practical Guide

  • Kara J. BlackerEmail author
  • Joseph Hamilton
  • Grant Roush
  • Kyle A. Pettijohn
  • Adam T. Biggs
Review

Abstract

In recent years, the potential to improve cognitive skills through training has captured the attention of academic researchers, the commercial market, and the general public. Numerous clinical and healthy populations have been identified as targets for cognitive training, and military personnel are one particular group that may be able to uniquely benefit from cognitive training interventions. Military operations involve a wide range of human performance skills, many of which are cognitive in nature. Use of cognitive training to improve these critical everyday skills for service members represents an untapped potential resource by which to improve operational readiness and warfighter performance. While much of the cognitive training research to date has been circumscribed within basic science pursuits, here we propose ways in which this research may start to be applied in a military setting. In the current review, we examine instances of military operations that may readily lend themselves to cognitive training. Further, we examine the existing literature from academic endeavors and pinpoint areas of exemplary efforts that can serve as a guide for military research to follow, as well as pitfalls to avoid. In particular, we identify and review evidence from the video game, working memory, and executive function training literatures. Finally, the goals of basic and applied science often differ, and that is certainly the case when comparing outcome-based research in a military context with mechanism-based research in an academic context. Therefore, we provide a guide for best practices when conducting cognitive training research specifically in a military setting. While cognitive training has attracted much controversy in both academia and commercial markets, we argue that utilizing near transfer effects in a targeted, outcome-based approach may represent a powerful tool to improve human performance in a number of military-relevant scenarios.

Keywords

ᅟ 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The views expressed in this article are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, nor the U.S. Government.

Funding

This work was supported by an Office of Naval Research award H1602 to ATB.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Copyright Statement

LT Adam T. Biggs is a military service member. This work was prepared as part of his official duties. Title 17 U.S.C. §105 provides that “Copyright protection under this title is not available for any work of the United States Government.” Title 17 U.S.C. §101 defines a U.S. Government work as a work prepared by a military service member or employee of the U.S. Government as part of that person’s official duties.

References

  1. Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: the same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 30–60.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30.Google Scholar
  2. Agay, N., Yechiam, E., Carmel, Z., & Levkovitz, Y. (2010). Non-specific effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin) on cognitive ability and decision-making of ADHD and healthy adults. Psychopharmacology, 210(4), 511–519.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1853-4.Google Scholar
  3. Alderton, D. L., Wolfe, J. H., & Larson, G. E. (1997). The ECAT battery. Military Psychology, 9, 5–37.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp0901_1.Google Scholar
  4. Allom, V., & Mullan, B. (2015). Two inhibitory control training interventions designed to improve eating behaviour and determine mechanisms of change. Appetite, 89, 282–290.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.022.Google Scholar
  5. Arciniegas, D. B., Held, K., & Wagner, P. (2002). Cognitive impairment following traumatic brain injury. Current Treatment Options in Neurology, 4(1), 43–57.Google Scholar
  6. Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 366–377.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x.Google Scholar
  7. Barman, A., Chatterjee, A., & Bhide, R. (2016). Cognitive impairment and rehabilitation strategies after traumatic brain injury. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine, 38(3), 172–181.  https://doi.org/10.4103/0253-7176.183086.Google Scholar
  8. Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637.Google Scholar
  9. Battleday, R. M., & Brem, A. K. (2015). Modafinil for cognitive neuroenhancement in healthy non-sleep-deprived subjects: a systematic review. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(11), 1865–1881.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.07.028.Google Scholar
  10. Bavelier, D., Green, C. S., Pouget, A., & Schrater, P. (2012). Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 391–416.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152832.Google Scholar
  11. Bejjanki, V. R., Zhang, R., Li, R., Pouget, A., Green, C. S., Lu, Z. L., & Bavelier, D. (2014). Action video game play facilitates the development of better perceptual templates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(47), 16961–16966.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1417056111.Google Scholar
  12. Berryhill, M. E., & Jones, K. T. (2012). tDCS selectively improves working memory in older adults with more education. Neuroscience Letters, 521(2), 148–151.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.074.Google Scholar
  13. Berryhill, M. E., Peterson, D. J., Jones, K. T., & Stephens, J. A. (2014). Hits and misses: leveraging tDCS to advance cognitive research. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 800.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00800.Google Scholar
  14. Biggs, A. T., Cain, M. S., & Mitroff, S. R. (2015). Cognitive training can reduce civilian casualties in a simulated shooting environment. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1164–1176.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615579274.Google Scholar
  15. Blacker, K. J., Curby, K. M., Klobusicky, E., & Chein, J. M. (2014). The effects of action video game training on visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(5), 1992–2004.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037556.Google Scholar
  16. Blacker, K. J., Negoita, S., Ewen, J. B., & Courtney, S. M. (2017). N-back versus complex span working memory training. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(4), 434–454.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0044-1.Google Scholar
  17. Bogdanova, Y., & Verfaellie, M. (2012). Cognitive sequelae of blast-induced traumatic brain injury: recovery and rehabilitation. Neuropsychological Review, 22(1), 4–20.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-012-9192-3.Google Scholar
  18. Boot, W. R., Simons, D. J., Stothart, C., & Stutts, C. (2013). The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 445–454.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613491271.Google Scholar
  19. Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 76–106). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Brawley, A. M., & Pury, C. S. (2016). It's like doing a job analysis: You know more about qualitative methods than you may think. Industrial & Organizational Psychology, 9(4), 753–760.Google Scholar
  21. Buschkuehl, M., Hernandez-Garcia, L., Jaeggi, S. M., Bernard, J. A., & Jonides, J. (2014). Neural effects of short-term training on working memory. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 147–160.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0244-9.Google Scholar
  22. Cantor, J., & Engle, R. W. (1993). Working-memory capacity as long-term memory activation: an individual-differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1101–1114.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1101.Google Scholar
  23. Chein, J. M., & Morrison, A. B. (2010). Expanding the mind's workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 193–199.  https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.2.193.Google Scholar
  24. Chen, Z., Veling, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Holland, R. W. (2016). How does not responding to appetitive stimuli cause devaluation: evaluative conditioning or response inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1687–1701.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000236.Google Scholar
  25. Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3), 449–464.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003.Google Scholar
  26. Chisholm, J. D., & Kingstone, A. (2012). Improved top-down control reduces oculomotor capture: the case of action video game players. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(2), 257–262.  https://doi.org/10.3758/S13414-011-0253-0.Google Scholar
  27. Chisholm, J. D., Hickey, C., Theeuwes, J., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Reduced attentional capture in action video game players. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 667–671.  https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.3.667.Google Scholar
  28. Chu, D. (2007). Qualification standards for enlistment, appointment, and induction. Retrieved from Washington, DC:Google Scholar
  29. Ciuffreda, K. J., Levi, D. M., & Selenow, A. (1991). Amblyopia: Basic and clinical aspects: Butterworth-Heinemann.Google Scholar
  30. Cogan, A., Madey, J., Kaufman, W., Holmlund, G., & Bach-y-Rita, P. (1977). Pong game as a rehabilitation device. Paper presented at the conference on systems and devices for the disabled, Seattle: University of Washington School of Medicine.Google Scholar
  31. Colom, R., Roman, F. J., Abad, F. J., Shih, P. C., Privado, J., Froufe, M.,. .. Jaeggi, S. M. (2013). Adaptive n-back training does not improve fluid intelligence at the construct level: Gains on individual tests suggest that training may enhance visuospatial processing. Intelligence, 41(5), 712–727. doi:  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.Intell.2013.09.002.Google Scholar
  32. Colzato, L. S., van Leeuwen, P. J. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Hommel, B. (2010). DOOM'd to switch: superior cognitive flexibility in players of first person shooter games. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–5.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00008.Google Scholar
  33. Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(12), 547–552.Google Scholar
  34. Council, N. R. (2015). Measuring human capabilities: an agenda for basic research on the assessment of individual and group performance potential for military accession. Washington, DC: The National Academic Press.Google Scholar
  35. Creswell, J. W. (2007). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five approaches. Thousand oaks, C.A.: Sage.Google Scholar
  36. Dale, G., & Green, C. S. (2017). The changing face of video games and video gamers: future directions in the scientific study of video game play and cognitive performance. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1–15.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0015-6.
  37. Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.Google Scholar
  38. Deveau, J., Jaeggi, S. M., Zordan, V., Phung, C., & Seitz, A. R. (2015). How to build better memory training games. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 243.Google Scholar
  39. DoD Worldwide Numbers for TBI. (2018). Retrieved from http://dvbic.dcoe.mil/dod-worldwide-numbers-tbi
  40. Dreary, I. (2008). Why do intelligent people live longer? Nature, 456(7219), 175–176.Google Scholar
  41. Dye, M. W. G., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Increasing speed of processing with action video games. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 321–326.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01660.x.Google Scholar
  42. Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.128.3.309.Google Scholar
  43. Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J., Figge, C., & Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Self-regulation of frontal-midline theta facilitates memory updating and mental set shifting. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 420.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00420.Google Scholar
  44. Fay, T. B., Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Bangert, B., Dietrich, A., Nuss, K. E.,. .. Wright, M. (2010). Cognitive reserve as a moderator of postconcussive symptoms in children with complicated and uncomplicated mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(1), 94–105. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709991007.Google Scholar
  45. Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18(10), 850–855.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01990.x.Google Scholar
  46. Foroughi, C. K., Monfort, S. S., Paczynski, M., McKnight, P. E., & Greenwood, P. M. (2016). Placebo effects in cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(27), 7470–7474.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601243113.Google Scholar
  47. Foster, J. L., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Draheim, C., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2017). Do the effects of working memory training depend on baseline ability level? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(11), 1677–1689.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000426.Google Scholar
  48. Gilleen, J., Michalopoulou, P. G., Reichenberg, A., Drake, R., Wykes, T., Lewis, S. W., & Kapur, S. (2014). Modafinil combined with cognitive training is associated with improved learning in healthy volunteers—a randomised controlled trial. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(4), 529–539.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.01.001.Google Scholar
  49. Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience, 6(4), 348–357.  https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.6.4.348.Google Scholar
  50. Gozli, D. G., Bavelier, D., & Pratt, J. (2014). The effect of action video game playing on sensorimotor learning: evidence from a movement tracking task. Human Movement Science, 38C, 152–162.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.09.004.Google Scholar
  51. Grafman, J., Jonas, B., & Salazar, A. (1990). Wisconsin card sorting test performance based on location and size of neuroanatomical lesion in Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injury. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71(3 Pt 2), 1120–1122.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1990.71.3f.1120.Google Scholar
  52. Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam head injury study. Neurology, 46(5), 1231–1238.Google Scholar
  53. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature, 423, 534–538.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01647.Google Scholar
  54. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Effect of action video games on spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1465–1478.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1465.Google Scholar
  55. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2007). Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychological Science, 18(1), 88–94.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01853.x.Google Scholar
  56. Green, C. S., Sugarman, M. A., Medford, K., Klobusicky, E., & Bavelier, D. (2012). The effect of action video game experience on task-switching. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3), 984–994.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.12.020.Google Scholar
  57. Green, C. S., Gorman, T., & Bavelier, D. (2016). Action video-game training and its effects on perception and attentional control. In T. Strobach & J. Karbach (Eds.), Cognitive training (pp. 107–116). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
  58. Groswasser, Z., Reider, G., II, Schwab, K., Ommaya, A. K., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R.,. .. Salazar, A. M. (2002). Quantitative imaging in late TBI. Part II: cognition and work after closed and penetrating head injury: a report of the Vietnam head injury study. Brain Injury, 16(8), 681–690. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050110119835.Google Scholar
  59. Gruzelier, J. H. (2014). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. I: a review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Review, 44, 124–141.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.015.Google Scholar
  60. Guerrieri, R., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2012). Disinhibition is easier learned than inhibition. The effects of (dis)inhibition training on food intake. Appetite, 59(1), 96–99.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.04.006.Google Scholar
  61. Haaland, K. Y., Sadek, J. R., Keller, J. E., & Castillo, D. T. (2016). Neurocognitive correlates of successful treatment of PTSD in female veterans. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(6), 643–651.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000424.Google Scholar
  62. Hallett, P. E. (1978). Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions. Vision Research, 18(10), 1279–1296.Google Scholar
  63. Hamilton, J. A., Lambert, G., Suss, J., & Biggs, A. T. (2018). Can cognitive training imprive shoot/don’t-shoot performance with live ammunition? Preliminary evidence from live fire exercises.Google Scholar
  64. Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Doppelmayr, M., Schabus, M., & Klimesch, W. (2005). Increasing individual upper alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects. Applied Psychophysiolology & Biofeedback, 30(1), 1–10.Google Scholar
  65. Haut, K. M., Lim, K. O., & MacDonald 3rd., A. (2010). Prefrontal cortical changes following cognitive training in patients with chronic schizophrenia: effects of practice, generalization, and specificity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(9), 1850–1859.  https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2010.52.Google Scholar
  66. Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2298.Google Scholar
  67. Hoedlmoser, K., Pecherstorfer, T., Gruber, G., Anderer, P., Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., & Schabus, M. (2008). Instrumental conditioning of human sensorimotor rhythm (12-15 Hz) and its impact on sleep as well as declarative learning. Sleep, 31(10), 1401–1408.Google Scholar
  68. Horn, J. L., & Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57(5), 253–270.Google Scholar
  69. Houben, K., & Jansen, A. (2011). Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations. Appetite, 56(2), 345–349.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.017.Google Scholar
  70. Houben, K., & Jansen, A. (2015). Chocolate equals stop. Chocolate-specific inhibition training reduces chocolate intake and go associations with chocolate. Appetite, 87, 318–323.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.005.Google Scholar
  71. Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Resisting temptation: Decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug & Alcohol Dependence, 116(1–3), 132–136.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011.Google Scholar
  72. Houben, K., Havermans, R. C., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2012). Beer a no-go: learning to stop responding to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced affective associations rather than increased response inhibition. Addiction, 107(7), 1280–1287.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03827.x.Google Scholar
  73. Huckans, M., Pavawalla, S., Demadura, T., Kolessar, M., Seelye, A., Roost, N.,. .. Storzbach, D. (2010). A pilot study examining effects of group-based Cognitive Strategy Training treatment on self-reported cognitive problems, psychiatric symptoms, functioning, and compensatory strategy use in OIF/OEF combat veterans with persistent mild cognitive disorder and history of traumatic brain injury. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 47(1), 43–60.Google Scholar
  74. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 105(19), 6829–6833.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801268105.
  75. Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y. F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning - implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.Intell.2010.09.001.Google Scholar
  76. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(25), 10081–10086.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1103228108.Google Scholar
  77. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464–480.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0364-z.Google Scholar
  78. Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54–64.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018438.Google Scholar
  79. Jones, A., & Field, M. (2013). The effects of cue-specific inhibition training on alcohol consumption in heavy social drinkers. Experimental Clinical Psychopharmacology, 21(1), 8–16.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030683.Google Scholar
  80. Jongkees, B. J., Hommel, B., Kuhn, S., & Colzato, L. S. (2015). Effect of tyrosine supplementation on clinical and healthy populations under stress or cognitive demands—a review. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 70, 50–57.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.08.014.Google Scholar
  81. Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637–671.Google Scholar
  82. Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978–990.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00846.x.Google Scholar
  83. Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 390.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00390.Google Scholar
  84. Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2027–2037.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614548725.Google Scholar
  85. Karle, J. W., Watter, S., & Shedden, J. M. (2010). Task switching in video game players: Benefits of selective attention but not resistance to proactive interference. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 70–78.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.007.Google Scholar
  86. Keizer, A. W., Verment, R. S., & Hommel, B. (2010). Enhancing cognitive control through neurofeedback: a role of gamma-band activity in managing episodic retrieval. NeuroImage, 49(4), 3404–3413.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.023.Google Scholar
  87. Kesler, S. R., Adams, H. F., Blasey, C. M., & Bigler, E. D. (2003). Premorbid intellectual functioning, education, and brain size in traumatic brain injury: an investigation of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Applied Neuropsychology, 10(3), 153–162.  https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN1003_04.Google Scholar
  88. Klimesch, W., Schack, B., & Sauseng, P. (2005). The functional significance of theta and upper alpha oscillations. Experimental Psychology, 52(2), 99–108.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.52.2.99.Google Scholar
  89. Konen, T., Strobach, T., & Karbach, J. (2016). Working memory. In T. Strobach & J. Karbach (Eds.), Cognitive training: An overview of features and applications. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
  90. van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Veling, H., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2014). Comparing two psychological interventions in reducing impulsive processes of eating behaviour: effects on self-selected portion size. British Journal of Health Psychology, 19(4), 767–782.  https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12075.Google Scholar
  91. Kray, J., Karbach, J., Haenig, S., & Freitag, C. (2011). Can task-switching training enhance executive control functioning in children with attention deficit/−hyperactivity disorder? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 180.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00180.Google Scholar
  92. Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 148–161.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148.Google Scholar
  93. Kuo, M. F., & Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 43(3), 192–199.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059412444975.Google Scholar
  94. Larson, G. E., & Saccuzzo, D. P. (1989). Cognitive correlates of general intelligence: toward a process theory of g. Intelligence, 13, 5–31.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(89)90003-2.Google Scholar
  95. Lawrence, N. S., Verbruggen, F., Morrison, S., Adams, R. C., & Chambers, C. D. (2015). Stopping to food can reduce intake. Effects of stimulus-specificity and individual differences in dietary restraint. Appetite, 85, 91–103.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.006.Google Scholar
  96. Lee, J., & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 599–611.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599.Google Scholar
  97. Li, S. C., Schmiedek, F., Huxhold, O., Rocke, C., Smith, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychological Aging, 23(4), 731–742.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014343.Google Scholar
  98. Li, R., Polat, U., Makous, W., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training. Nature Neuroscience, 12(5), 549–551.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2296.Google Scholar
  99. Li, R., Polat, U., Scalzo, F., & Bavelier, D. (2010). Reducing backward masking through action game training. Journal of Vision, 10(14).  https://doi.org/10.1167/10.14.33.
  100. Li, C. H., He, X., Wang, Y. J., Hu, Z., & Guo, C. Y. (2017). Visual working memory capacity can be increased by training on distractor filtering efficiency. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 196.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00196.Google Scholar
  101. Light, G. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2015). Future clinical uses of neurophysiological biomarkers to predict and monitor treatment response for schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1344, 105–119.  https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12730.Google Scholar
  102. Limitation on enlistment and induction of persons whose score on the armed forces qualification test is below a prescribed level, § 520 (2011).Google Scholar
  103. Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users' guide to the stop signal paradigm.Google Scholar
  104. Lovden, M., Backman, L., Lindenberger, U., Schaefer, S., & Schmiedek, F. (2010). A theoretical framework for the study of adult cognitive plasticity. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 659–676.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020080.Google Scholar
  105. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279–281.  https://doi.org/10.1038/36846.Google Scholar
  106. Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 12(4), 163–169.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005.Google Scholar
  107. Machizawa, M. G., & Driver, J. (2011). Principal component analysis of behavioural individual differences suggests that particular aspects of visual working memory may relate to specific aspects of attention. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1518–1526.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.032.Google Scholar
  108. McDermott, T. J., Badura-Brack, A. S., Becker, K. M., Ryan, T. J., Bar-Haim, Y., Pine, D. S.,. .. Wilson, T. W. (2016). Attention training improves aberrant neural dynamics during working memory processing in veterans with PTSD Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(6), 1140–1149. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0459-7.Google Scholar
  109. Melby-Lervag, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Science, 49(2), 270–291.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.Google Scholar
  110. Minear, M., & Shah, P. (2008). Training and transfer effects in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 36(8), 1470–1483.  https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.336.8.1470.Google Scholar
  111. Mishra, J., Zinni, M., Bavelier, D., & Hillyard, S. A. (2011). Neural basis of superior performance of action videogame players in an attention-demanding task. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(3), 992–998.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4834-10.2011.Google Scholar
  112. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.  https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.Google Scholar
  113. Mogle, J. A., Lovett, B. J., Stawski, R. S., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2008). What's so special about working memory? An examination of the relationships among working memory, secondary memory, and fluid intelligence. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1071–1077.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02202.x.Google Scholar
  114. Mohammed, S., Flores, L., Deveau, J., Hoffing, R. C., Phung, C., Parlett, C. M.,. .. Seitz, A. R. (2017). The benefits and challenges of implementing motivational features to boost cognitive training outcome. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(4), 491–507.Google Scholar
  115. Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(3), 134–140.Google Scholar
  116. Moore, W., Pedlow, S., Krishnamurty, P., Wolter, K., & Chicago, I. L. (2000). National longitudinal survey of youth 1997 (NLSY97). Retrieved from Chicago, IL:Google Scholar
  117. Morgeson, F. P., & Dierdorff, E. C. (2011). Work analysis: From technique to theory. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 3–41). Washington, D.C.: APA.Google Scholar
  118. Morrison, A. B., & Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 46–60.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0034.Google Scholar
  119. Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. The Journal of Physiology, 527(Pt 3), 633–639.Google Scholar
  120. Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology, 57(10), 1899–1901.Google Scholar
  121. Parkin, B. L., Ekhtiari, H., & Walsh, V. F. (2015). Non-invasive human brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a primer. Neuron, 87(5), 932–945.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.032.Google Scholar
  122. Patrick, J., & Moore, A. K. (1985). Development and reliability of a job analysis technique. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 58(2), 149–158.Google Scholar
  123. Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., & Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: a meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1055–1079.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0418-z.Google Scholar
  124. Rasmussen, R. E. (2007). The wrong target: the problem of mistargeting resulting in fratricide and civilian casualties. National Defense University Norfolk VA Joint Advanced Warfighting School.Google Scholar
  125. Raymont, V., Salazar, A. M., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2011). Studying injured minds—the Vietnam head injury study and 40 years of brain injury research. Frontiers in Neurology, 2, 15.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2011.00015.Google Scholar
  126. Redick, T. S. (2016). On the relation of working memory and multitasking: memory span and synthetic work performance. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(4), 401–409.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j/jarmac.2016.05.003.Google Scholar
  127. Reisberg, D., Baron, J., & Kemler, D. G. (1980). Overcoming Stroop interference: the effects of practice on distractor potency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 6(1), 140–150.Google Scholar
  128. Richardson, J. M. (2016). A design for maintaining maritime superiority. Naval War College Review, 69(2), 11–18.Google Scholar
  129. Richmond, L. L., Morrison, A. B., Chein, J. M., & Olson, I. R. (2011). Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychological Aging, 26(4), 813–822.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023631.Google Scholar
  130. Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(41), 14931–14936.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506897102.Google Scholar
  131. Russell, T. L., Ford, L., & Ramsberger, P. (2014). Thoughts on the future of military enlisted selection and classification. Retrieved from Alexandria, VA:Google Scholar
  132. Sager, C. E., Peterson, N. G., Oppler, S. H., Rosse, R. L., & Walker, C. B. (1997). An examination of five indexes of test battery performance: analysis of the ECAT battery. Military Psychology, 9, 97–120.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp0901_6.Google Scholar
  133. Sandry, J., DeLuca, J., & Chiaravalloti, N. (2015). Working memory capacity links cognitive reserve with long-term memory in moderate to severe TBI: a translational approach. Journal of Neurology, 262(1), 59–64.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-014-7523-4.Google Scholar
  134. Schmiedek, F., Lovden, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Hundred Days of Cognitive Training Enhance Broad Cognitive Abilities in Adulthood: Findings from the COGITO Study. Front Aging Neurosci, 2. doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2010.00027.
  135. Schneider, E. B., Sur, S., Raymont, V., Duckworth, J., Kowalski, R. G., Efron, D. T.,. .. Stevens, R. D. (2014). Functional recovery after moderate/severe traumatic brain injury: a role for cognitive reserve? Neurology, 82(18), 1636–1642. doi: https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000000379.Google Scholar
  136. Schneiders, J. A., Opitz, B., Krick, C. M., & Mecklinger, A. (2011). Separating intra-modal and across-modal training effects in visual working memory: an fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex, 21(11), 2555–2564.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr037.Google Scholar
  137. Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Buhner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138–166.Google Scholar
  138. Shamosh, N. A., Deyoung, C. G., Green, A. E., Reis, D. L., Johnson, M. R., Conway, A. R.,. .. Gray, J. R. (2008). Individual differences in delay discounting: relation to intelligence, working memory, and anterior prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 19(9), 904–911. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02175.x.Google Scholar
  139. Sherlin, L. H., Arns, M., Lubar, J., Heinrich, H., Kerson, C., Strehl, U., & al., e. (2011). Neurofeedback and basic learning theory: implications for research and practice. Journal of Neurotherapy, 15, 292–304. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/10874208.2011.623089.
  140. Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is Working Memory Training Effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654.  https://doi.org/10.1037/A0027473.Google Scholar
  141. Silver, H., Feldman, P., Bilker, W., & Gur, R. C. (2003). Working memory deficit as a core neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(10), 1809–1816.  https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1809.Google Scholar
  142. Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. (2016). Do "brain-training" programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616661983.Google Scholar
  143. Singh, B., Cable, G. G., Hampson, G. V., Pascoe, G. D., Corbett, M., & Smith, A. (2010). Hypoxia awareness training for aircrew: a comparison of two techniques. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 81(9), 857–863.Google Scholar
  144. Smith, A. M. (2008). Hypoxia symptoms in military aircrew: long-term recall vs. acute experience in training. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 79(1), 54–57.Google Scholar
  145. Stagg, C. J., & Nitsche, M. A. (2011). Physiological basis of transcranial direct current stimulation. The Neuroscientist, 17(1), 37–53.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410386614.Google Scholar
  146. Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(3), 448–460.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702813248.Google Scholar
  147. Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47(10), 2015–2028.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004.Google Scholar
  148. Strobach, T., & Karbach, J. (2016). Cognitive training: An overview of features and applications. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
  149. Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., & Schubert, T. (2012). Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 13–24.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.Actpsy.2012.02.001.Google Scholar
  150. Strobach, T., Salminen, T., Karbach, J., & Schubert, T. (2014). Practice-related optimization and transfer of executive functions: a general review and a specific realization of their mechanisms in dual tasks. Psychological Research, 78(6), 836–851.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0563-7.Google Scholar
  151. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.Google Scholar
  152. Tang, Y. Y., Holzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 213–225.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3916.Google Scholar
  153. Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S., Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106–113.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x.Google Scholar
  154. Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficicency of the other functions. (I). Psychological Review, 8(3).Google Scholar
  155. Tuholski, S. W., Engle, R. W., & Baylis, G. C. (2001). Individual differences in working memory capacity and enumeration. Memory & Cognition, 29(3), 484–492.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196399.Google Scholar
  156. Twamley, E. W., Jak, A. J., Delis, D. C., Bondi, M. W., & Lohr, J. B. (2014). Cognitive symptom management and rehabilitation therapy (CogSMART) for veterans with traumatic brain injury: pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Rehabilitation Research & Development, 51(1), 59–70.  https://doi.org/10.1682/JRRD.2013.01.0020.Google Scholar
  157. Unsworth, N., Fukuda, K., Awh, E., & Vogel, E. K. (2014). Working memory delay activity predicts individual differences in cognitive abilities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1–13.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00765.
  158. Vartanian, O., Coady, L., & Blackler, K. (2016). 3D multiple object tracking boosts working memory span: Implications for cognitive training in military populations. Military Psychology, 28(5), 353–360.Google Scholar
  159. Vedamurthy, I., Nahum, M., Bavelier, D., & Levi, D. M. (2015a). Mechanisms of recovery of visual function in adult amblyopia through a tailored action video game. Scientific Reports, 5, 8482.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep08482.Google Scholar
  160. Vedamurthy, I., Nahum, M., Huang, S. J., Zheng, F., Bayliss, J., Bavelier, D., & Levi, D. M. (2015b). A dichoptic custom-made action video game as a treatment for adult amblyopia. Vision Research, 114, 173–187.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2015.04.008.Google Scholar
  161. Veling, H., Aarts, H., & Stroebe, W. (2013). Using stop signals to reduce impulsive choices for palatable unhealthy foods. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18(2), 354–368.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8287.2012.02092.x.Google Scholar
  162. Veling, H., van Koningsbruggen, G. M., Aarts, H., & Stroebe, W. (2014). Targeting impulsive processes of eating behavior via the internet. Effects on body weight. Appetite, 78, 102–109.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.03.014.Google Scholar
  163. Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 649–672.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013170.Google Scholar
  164. Vogel, E. K., McCollough, A. W., & Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature, 438(24), 500–503.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04171.Google Scholar
  165. Waller, D. C. (1994). The commandos: the inside story of America's secret soldiers: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
  166. Wilson, K. M., Head, J., & Helton, W. S. (2013). Friendly fire in a simulated firearms task. Paper presented at the human factors and ergonomics society, Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
  167. Wilson, K. M., Head, J., de Joux, N. R., Finkbeiner, K. M., & Helton, W. S. (2015). Friendly fire and the sustained attention to response task. Human Factors, 57(7), 1219–1234.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720815605703.Google Scholar
  168. Zinke, K., Zeintl, M., Eschen, A., Herzog, C., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Potentials and limits of plasticity induced by working memory training in old-old age. Gerontology, 58(1), 79–87.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000324240.Google Scholar
  169. Zinke, K., Zeintl, M., Rose, N. S., Putzmann, J., Pydde, A., & Kliegel, M. (2014). Working memory training and transfer in older adults: effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 304–315.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032982.Google Scholar
  170. Zoefel, B., Huster, R. J., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1427–1431.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.078.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© This is a U.S. government work and its text is not subject to copyright protection in the United States; however, its text may be subject to foreign copyright protection  2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kara J. Blacker
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Joseph Hamilton
    • 3
  • Grant Roush
    • 1
    • 2
  • Kyle A. Pettijohn
    • 1
    • 2
  • Adam T. Biggs
    • 2
  1. 1.The Henry M. Jackson Foundation for the Advancement of Military Medicine IncBethesdaUSA
  2. 2.Naval Medical Research Unit DaytonWright-Patterson AFBUSA
  3. 3.Randolph Macon CollegeAshlandUSA

Personalised recommendations