Advertisement

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

, Volume 4, Issue 4, pp 329–343 | Cite as

Effects of Brief Interactions with Male Experimenters Shortly Before and During the Trier Social Stress Test on Study Participants’ Testosterone Salivary Concentrations

  • Eunhye Kim
  • Nora Nickels
  • Dario Maestripieri
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
  • 37 Downloads

Abstract

Objectives

In the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST), participants briefly interact with a research assistant (‘greeter’) prior to the test, and then with two ‘judges’ (a ‘talking judge’ and a ‘timing judge’) during the test. We investigated the effects of the sex of the greeter and sex of the judges on participants’ pre-test and post-test salivary concentrations of testosterone and cortisol.

Procedures

Study participants were 137 male and 31 female university students, who underwent the TSST in our laboratory.

Results

The TSST was associated with a significant increase in cortisol across male and female participants, and also with an increase in testosterone among male, but not female, participants. Male participants who interacted with a male greeter prior to the TSST had significantly higher pre-test testosterone levels than males who interacted with a female greeter. Moreover, both male and female participants who had a male ‘talking’ judge and a female ‘timing’ judge had significantly greater testosterone increases after the TSST compared to participants who had a female talking judge and a male timing judge, or two female judges. No effects of sex of the greeter or sex of either judge on cortisol levels were found.

Conclusions

The most likely explanation for the effects of male greeters and male talking judges on participants’ testosterone levels is that interacting with these individuals stimulated competitiveness in the participants. This finding contributes to the growing literature documenting adaptive hormonal changes in response to brief social interactions with individuals of the same or the opposite sex and also has methodological implications for studies using the TSST.

Keywords

Testosterone Cortisol Stress Trier social stress test Sex Competition 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank Amanda Dobbyn, Andrea Henry, Konrad Kubicki, Davide Ponzi, Jonathan Seiden, Alex Voss, Melissa Infosino, Martha Alvarez, Alex Cordover, Sumer Vaid, and Staci Gusakova for their assistance with data collection. We thank James Roney for helpful comments on the manuscript. The authors have full control of all primary data and the University of Chicago played no role in data collection, analysis, and interpretation.

Funding

This study was supported by intramural funds from the University of Chicago.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval and Informed Consent

Written informed consent was obtained from all study participants. All experimental procedures were approved by the Social Science Institutional Review Board at the University of Chicago.

References

  1. Andrews, J., Wadiwalla, M., Juster, R. P., Lord, C., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2007). Effects of manipulating the amount of social-evaluative threat on the cortisol stress response in young healthy men. Behavioural Neuroscience, 121(5), 871–876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. van Anders, S., & Watson, N. V. (2007). Social neuroendocrinology. Human Nature, 17(2), 212–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 319–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bateup, H. S., Booth, A., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Granger, D. A. (2002). Testosterone, cortisol, and women's competition. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(3), 181–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bechtoldt, M. N., & Schneider, V. K. (2016). Predicting stress from the ability to eavesdrop on feelings: Emotional intelligence and testosterone jointly predict cortisol reactivity. Emotion, 16(6), 815–825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boksem, M. S., Kostermans, E., Milivojevic, B., & Cremer, D. D. (2012). Social status determines how we monitor and evaluate our performance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(3), 304–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dabbs, J. M., Carr, T. S., Frady, R. L., & Riad, J. K. (1995). Testosterone, crime, and misbehavior among 692 male prison inmates. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(5), 627–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Duchesne, A., Tessera, E., Dedovic, K., Engert, V., & Pressier, J. C. (2012). Effects of panel sex composition on the physiological stress responses to psychosocial stress in healthy young men and women. Biological Psychology, 89, 99–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Duehr, E. E., & Bono, J. E. (2006). Men, women, and managers: Are stereotypes finally changing? Personnel Psychology, 59, 815–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ellison, P. T., & Gray, P. B. (Eds.). (2009). Endocrinology of social relationships. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010). Oxytocin, cortisol, and triadic family interactions. Physiology & Behavior, 101(5), 679–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gray, P. B., Campbell, B. C., Marlowe, F. W., Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (2004). Social variables predict between-subject but not day-to-day variation in the testosterone of US men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(9), 1153–1162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gruenewald, T. L., Kemeny, M. E., & Aziz, N. (2006). Subjective social status moderates cortisol responses to social threat. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 20(4), 410–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Heilman, M. E., Block, C. K., Martell, R. F., & Simon, M. C. (1989). Has anything changed? Current characteristics of men, women, and managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 935–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Henry, A., Sattizhan, J. R., Norman, G. J., Beilock, S. L., & Maestripieri, D. (2017). Performance during competition and competition outcome in relation to testosterone and cortisol among women. Hormones and Behavior, 92, 82–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Juster, R. P., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mendrek, A., Pfaus, J. G., Smith, N. G., Johnson, P. J., Lefebvre-Louis, J., Raymond, C., Marin, M., Sindi, S., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2015). Sexual orientation modulates endocrine stress reactivity. Biological Psychiatry, 77(7), 668–676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The 'Trier social stress Test': A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 76–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. López, H. H., Hay, A. C., & Conklin, P. H. (2009). Attractive men induce testosterone and cortisol release in women. Hormones and Behavior, 56(1), 84–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Liu, J. J. W., Ein, N., Peck, K., Huang, V., Pruessner, J. C., & Vickers, K. (2017). Sex differences in salivary cortisol reactivity to the trier social stress test (TSST): A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 82, 26–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Maestripieri, D., Baran, N. M., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2010). Between- and within-sex variation in hormonal responses to psychological stress in a large sample of college students. Stress, 13, 413–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Mazur, A. (1985). A biosocial model of status in face-to-face primate groups. Social Forces, 64(2), 377–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(3), 353–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mehta, P. H., & Josephs, R. A. (2010). Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: Evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 898–906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mehta, P. H., Jones, A. C., & Josephs, R. A. (2008). The social endocrinology of dominance: Basal testosterone predicts cortisol changes and behavior following victory and defeat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1078–1093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Miller, S. L., Maner, J. K., & McNulty, J. K. (2012). Adaptive attunement to the sex of individuals at a competition: The ratio of opposite- to same-sex individuals correlates with changes in competitors' testosterone levels. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(1), 57–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Morris, M. C., Kouros, C. D., Mielock, A. S., & Rao, U. (2017). Depressive symptom composites associated with cortisol stress reactivity in adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 210, 181–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nickels, N., Kubicki, K., & Maestripieri, D. (2017). Sex differences in the effects of psychosocial stress on cooperative and prosocial behavior: Evidence for ‘flight-or-fight’ in males and ‘tend-and-befriend’ in females. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 3, 171–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ponzi, D., Henry, A., Kubicki, K., Nickels, N., Wilson, M. C., & Maestripieri, D. (2016). Autistic-like traits, socio-sexuality, and hormonal responses to socially stressful and sexually arousing stimuli in male students. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2, 150–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Prime, J. L., Carter, N. M., & Welbourne, T. M. (2009). Women 'take care,' men 'take charge': Managers' stereotypic perceptions of women and men leaders. The Psychologist- Manager Journal, 12(1), 25–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. van der Meij, L., Buunk, A. P., van de Sande, J. P., & Salvador, A. (2008). The presence of a woman increases testosterone in aggressive dominant men. Hormones and Behavior, 54(5), 640–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Romero-Martínez, Á., Lila, M., Sariñana-González, P., González-Bono, E., & Moya-Albiol, L. (2013). High testosterone levels and sensitivity to acute stress in perpetrators of domestic violence with low cognitive flexibility and impairments in their emotional decoding process: A preliminary study. Aggressive Behavior, 39(5), 355–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Roney, J. R. (2009). The role of sex hormones in the initiation of human mating relationships. In P. T. Ellison, P. B. Gray, P. T. Ellison, & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 246–269). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Roney, J. R., & Gettler, L. T. (2015). The role of testosterone in human romantic relationships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 81–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Roney, J. R., Mahler, S. V., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Behavioral and hormonal responses of men to brief interactions with women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(6), 365–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Roney, J. R., Lukaszewski, A. W., & Simmons, Z. L. (2007). Rapid endocrine responses of young men to social interactions with young women. Hormones and Behavior, 52(3), 326–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women's progress in management. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 675–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Smith, T. W., & Jordan, K. D. (2015). Interpersonal motives and social-evaluative threat: Effects of acceptance and status stressors on cardiovascular reactivity and salivary cortisol response. Psychophysiology, 52(2), 269–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wadiwalla, M., Andrews, J., Lai, B., Buss, C., Lupien, S. J., & Pruessner, J. C. (2010). Effects of manipulating the amount of social-evaluative threat on the cortisol stress response in young healthy women. Stress, 13(3), 214–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wingfield, J. C., Hegner, R. E., Dufty Jr., A. M., & Ball, G. F. (1990). The" challenge hypothesis": Theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies. The American Naturalist, 136(6), 829–846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wilson, M. C., Zilioli, S., Ponzi, D., Henry, A., Kubicki, K., Nickels, N., & Maestripieri, D. (2015). Cortisol reactivity to psychosocial stress mediates the relationship between extraversion and unrestricted sociosexuality. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Zilioli, S., & Watson, N. V. (2012). The hidden dimensions of the competition effect: Basal cortisol and basal testosterone jointly predict changes in salivary testosterone after social victory in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 1855–1865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zilioli, S., Ponzi, D., Henry, A., & Maestripieri, D. (2015). Testosterone, cortisol, and empathy: Evidence for the dual-hormone hypothesis. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1, 421–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Zilioli, S., Ponzi, D., Henry, A., Kubicki, K., Nickels, N., Wilson, M. C., & Maestripieri, D. (2016). Interest in babies negatively predicts testosterone responses to sexual visual stimuli among heterosexual young men. Psychological Science, 27(1), 114–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Comparative Human Development and Institute for Mind and BiologyThe University of ChicagoChicagoUSA

Personalised recommendations