Advertisement

The Study of Perspective-Taking: Contributions from Mainstream Psychology and Behavior Analysis

  • Deirdre KavanaghEmail author
  • Yvonne Barnes-Holmes
  • Dermot Barnes-Holmes
Theoretical Article

Abstract

The key purpose of this article is to provide a comprehensive overview of the literature on perspective-taking from within mainstream psychology and behavior analysis. The primary focus will be on the behavior-analytic approach to perspective-taking, which is divided into what may be described as (1) traditional behavior analysis; (2) an area within behavior analysis that is concerned specifically with human language and cognition, known as Relational Frame Theory (RFT); and (3) an updated version of RFT that is used to provide the beginnings of a detailed and systematic account of the processes involved in perspective-taking abilities.

Keywords

Relational frame theory perspective-taking false belief behavioral processes 

Notes

Funding

This conceptual paper was prepared with the funding from the FWO Type 1 Odysseus program at Ghent University, Belgium.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Deirdre Kavanagh declares that she has no conflict of interest. Yvonne Barnes-Holmes declares that she has no conflict of interest. Dermot Barnes-Holmes declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals by any of the authors.

References

  1. Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind reader’s tool kit: Projection and stereotyping. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87, 340–353.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andrews, G., Halford, G. S., Bunch, K. M., Bowden, D., & Jones, T. (2003). Theory of mind and relational complexity. Child Development, 74, 1476–1499.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00618.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Apperly, I. A., Back, E., Samson, D., & France, L. (2008). The cost of thinking about false beliefs: Evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task. Cognition, 106, 1093–1108.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Apperly, I. A., Riggs, K. J., Simpson, A., Chiavarino, C., & Samson, D. (2006). Is belief reasoning automatic? Psychological Science, 17, 841–844.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01791.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Arriaga, X. B., & Rusbult, C. E. (1998). Standing in my partner’s shoes: Partner perspective taking and reactions to accommodative dilemmas. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 928–948.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298249002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Arsenio, W. F. (2003). Emotional intelligence and the intelligence of emotions: A developmental perspective on mixed models. Human Development, 46, 97–103.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000068582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barbero-Rubio, A., López-López, J. C., Luciano, C., & Eisenbeck, N. (2016). Perspective-taking measured by implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP). The Psychological Record, 66, 243–252.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0166-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barnes-Holmes, D. (2018). The double-edged sword of human language and cognition: Shall we be Olympians or fallen angels? [blog post]. Behavior Science Dissemination: ABAI.Google Scholar
  9. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hussey, I., & Luciano, C. (2016). Relational frame theory: Finding its historical and philosophical roots and reflecting upon its future development: An introduction to part II. In R. D. Zettle, S. C. Hayes, P. M. D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of contextual behavioral science (pp. 117–128). West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.  https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118489857.ch8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Luciano, C., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). From the IRAP and REC model to a multi-dimensional multi-level framework for analysing the dynamics of arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6, 434–445.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Barnes-Holmes, D., Hayden, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2008). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a response-time and event-related-potentials methodology for testing natural verbal relations: A preliminary study. The Psychological Record, 58, 497–515 Retrieved from https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol58/iss4/1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2001). Analysing relational frames: Studying language and cognition in young children (Doctoral thesis). Maynooth: National University of Ireland.Google Scholar
  13. Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., Strand, P., & Friman, P. (2004). Establishing relational responding in accordance with more-than and less-than as generalized operant behavior in young children. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 4, 531–558.Google Scholar
  14. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Theory of mind in normal development and autism. Prisme, 34, 174–183.Google Scholar
  16. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistics child have a “theory of mind?”. Cognition, 21, 37–46.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K. (1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 29, 407–418.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023035012436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (2000). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Barron, B. F., Verkuylen, L., Belisle, J., Paliliunas, D., & Dixon, M. R. (2018). Teaching “then-later” and “here-there” relations to children with autism: An evaluation of single reversals and transformation of stimulus function. Behavior Analysis in Practice. Advance online publication. doi.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-018-0216-1.
  20. Belisle, J., Dixon, M. R., Stanley, C. R., Munoz, B., & Daar, J. H. (2016). Teaching foundational perspective-taking skills to children with autism using the PEAK-T curriculum: Single-reversal “I-you” deictic frames. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 965–969.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.324.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Berens, N. M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Arbitrarily applicable comparative relations: Experimental evidence for a relational operant. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 45–71.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.7-06.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., Lichtash, Y., Tanay, G., Shepherd, K., & Fresco, D. M. (2015). Decentering and related constructs: A critical review and metacognitive process model. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 599–617.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615594577.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Birch, S. A. J., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychological Science, 18, 382–386.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01909.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Borg, I., & Groenen, P. (2005). Modern multidimensional scaling: Theory and applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  25. Boucher, J. (2012). Putting theory of mind in its place: Psychological explanations of the socio-emotional-communicative impairments in autistic spectrum disorder. Autism, 16, 226–246.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311430403.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Bradford, E. E. F., Jentzsch, I., & Gomez, J. (2015). From self to social cognition: Theory of mind mechanisms and their relation to executive function. Cognition, 138, 21–34.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.02.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Brassil, N., Hyland, J., O’Hora, D., & Stewart, I. (2019). Reversing time and size: Mutual entailment of nonarbitrary temporal and magnitude relational responding. The Psychological Record, 69, 95–105.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0323-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Brino, A. L., Campos, R. S., Galvão, O. F., & McIlvane, W. J. (2014). Blank-comparison matching-to-sample reveals a false positive symmetry test in a capuchin monkey. Psychology & Neuroscience, 7, 193–198.  https://doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2014.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Broekhof, E., Ketelaar, L., Stockman, L., van Zijp, A., Bos, M. G., & Rieffe, C. (2015). The understanding of intentions, desires and beliefs in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 45, 2035–2045.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2363-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015). Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 67–78.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Bull, R., Phillips, L. H., & Conway, C. A. (2008). The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function. Cognition, 107, 663–672.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.015.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Carpendale, J. I., & Lewis, C. (2006). How children develop social understanding. Malden, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  34. Carr, D., Wilkinson, K. M., Blackman, D., & McIlvane, W. J. (2000). Equivalence classes in individuals with minimal verbal repertoires. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 101–114.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2000.74-101.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: A pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61, 173–198.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Charlop-Christy, M. J., & Daneshvar, S. (2003). Using video modelling to teach perspective taking to children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Intervention, 5, 12–21.  https://doi.org/10.1177/10983007030050010101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Colonnesi, C., Rieffe, C., Koops, W., & Perucchini, P. (2008). Precursors of a theory of mind: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 561–577.  https://doi.org/10.1348/026151008X285660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 5–13.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-G.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Cutting, A. L., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853–865. https://doi.org/10.1009-3920/99/7004-0004.Google Scholar
  40. Davis, M. H., Soderlund, T., Cole, J., Gadol, E., Kute, M., Myers, & Weihing, J. (2004). Cognitions associated with attempts to emphasize: How do we imagine the perspective of another? Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1625–1635.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Davlin, N. L., Rehfledt, R. A., & Lovett, S. (2011). A relational frame theory approach to understanding perspective-taking using children’s stories in typically developing children. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 12, 403–430.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2011.11434392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. DeBernardis, G. M., Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2014). Perspective taking as a continuum. The Psychological Record, 64, 123–131.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0008-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Dixon, M. (2014). PEAK relational training system: Direct training module. Carbondale, IL: Shawnee Scientific Press.Google Scholar
  44. Dixon, M. R., & Zlomke, K. M. (2005). Using the precursor to the relational evaluation procedure (PREP) to establish the relational frames of sameness, opposition, and distinction. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología, 37, 305–316.Google Scholar
  45. Dube, W. V., MacDonald, R. P. F., Mansfield, R. C., Holcomb, W. L., & Ahearn, W. H. (2004). Toward a behavioral analysis of joint attention. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 197–207.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393180.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 132–139.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Dunn, J., & Hughes, C. (1998). Young children’s understanding of emotions within close relationships. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 171–190.  https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Dunne, S., Foody, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Holmes, B., & Murphy, C. (2014). Facilitating repertoires of coordination, opposition, distinction, and comparison in young children with autism. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 37–47.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with same and opposite relational frames. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 249–262.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.88-249.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006). Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517–549). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  51. Epley, N. (2004). A tale of tuned decks? Anchoring as accessibility and anchoring as adjustment. In D. J. Koehler & N. Harvey (Eds.), Handbook of judgement and decision making (pp. 240–257). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Epley, N., & Caruso, E. M. (2009). Perspective-taking: Misstepping into others’ shoes. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 297–311). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  53. Epley, N., Caruso, E. M., & Bazerman, M. H. (2006). When perspective taking increases taking: Reactive egoism in social interaction. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 91, 872–889.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 17, 327–339.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Farrant, B. M., Devine, T. A., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2012). Empathy, perspective taking and prosocial behavior: The importance of parenting practices. Infant & Child Development, 21, 175–188.  https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Fertuck, E. A., Jekal, A., Song, I., Wyman, B., Morris, M. C., Wilson, S. T., et al. (2009). Enhanced “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” in borderline personality disorder compared to healthy controls. Psychological Medicine, 39, 1979–1988.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329170900600X.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2018). Exploring the single-trial-type-dominance-effect in the IRAP: Developing a differential arbitrarily applicable relational responding effects (DAARRE) Model. The Psychological Record, 68, 11–25.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0262-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Flavell, J. H. (1977). The development of knowledge about visual perception. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25, 43–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective-taking. In H. Beilin & P. B. Pufall (Eds.), Piaget’s theory: Prospects and possibilities. The Jean Piaget symposium series (Vol. 14, pp. 107–139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  60. Flavell, J. H. (2004). Theory-of-mind development: Retrospect and prospect. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50(3), 274–290 Retrieved from https://search.proquest.com/docview/230095187?accountid=11077.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavell, E. R. (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99–103.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.1.99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Flobbe, L., Verbrugge, R., Hendricks, P., & Krämer, I. (2008). Children’s application of theory of mind in reasoning and language. Journal of Logic, Language & Information, 17, 417–422.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-008-9064-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Fretland, R. A., Andeson, S., Sundet, K., Andreassen, O. A., Melle, I., & Vaskin, A. (2015). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: Error types and associations with symptoms. Schizophrenia Research, 162, 42–46.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.024.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Frick, A., Möhring, W., & Newcombe, N. S. (2014). Picturing perspectives: development of perspective-taking abilities in 4- to 8-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–7.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Perspectives on Disease, 14, 433–438.Google Scholar
  66. Galinsky, A. D., & Ku, G. (2004). The effects of perspective-taking on prejudice: The moderating role of self-evaluation. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 594–604.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Galinsky, A. D., Maddux, W. W., Gilin, D., & White, J. B. (2008). Why it pays to get inside the head of your opponent: The differential effects of perspective-taking and empathy in negotiations. Psychological Science, 19, 378–384.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02096.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Galinsky, A. D., & Mussweiler, T. (2001). First offers as anchors: The role of perspective-taking and negotiator focus. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 81, 657–669.  https://doi.org/10.1037//OO22-3514.81.4.657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. German, T. P., & Hehman, J. A. (2006). Representational and executive selection resources in “theory of mind”: Evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition, 101, 129–152.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Gil, E., Luciano, C., Ruiz, F. J., & Valdivia-Salas, S. (2012). A preliminary demonstration of transformation of functions through hierarchical relations. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 12, 1–19.Google Scholar
  71. Gil, E., Luciano, C., Ruiz, F. J., & Valdivia-Salas, S. (2014). A further experimental step in the analysis of hierarchical responding. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 14, 137–153.Google Scholar
  72. Gilroy, S. P., Lorah, E. R., Dodge, J., & Fiorello, C. (2015). Establishing deictic repertoires in autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 19, 82–92.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.04.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Giurfa, M., Zhang, S., Jenett, A., Menzel, R., & Srinivasan, M. V. (2001). The concepts of “sameness” and “difference” in an insect. Nature, 410, 930–933.  https://doi.org/10.1038/35073582.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Gómez-Becerra, I., Martín, M. J., Chávez-Brown, M., & Greer, R. D. (2007). Perspective taking in children with autism. European Journal of Behaviour Analysis, 8, 13–28.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2007.11434270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Gopnik, A., & Slaughter, V. (1991). Young children’s understanding of changes in their mental states. Child Development, 62, 98–110.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Gore, N. J., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Murphy, G. (2010). The relationship between intellectual functioning and relational perspective-taking. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 10, 1–17.Google Scholar
  77. Gorham, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Derived comparative and transitive relations in young children with and without autism. The Psychological Record, 59, 221–246.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Gould, E., Tarbox, J., O’Hora, D., Noone, S., & Bergstrom, R. (2011). Teaching children with autism a basic component skill of perspective-taking. Behavioral Interventions, 25, 50–66.  https://doi.org/10.1002/bin.320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Griffee, K., & Dougher, M. J. (2002). Contextual control of stimulus generalization and stimulus equivalence in hierarchical categorization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 433–447.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2002.78-433.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  80. Grosse Wiesmann, C., Schreiber, J., Singer, T., Steinbesi, N., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of Theory of Mind in early childhood. Nature Communications, 8, 1–10.  https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Guinther, P. M. (2017). Contextual influence over deriving others’ true beliefs using a relational triangulation perspective-taking protocol (RT-PTP-M1). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 108, 433–456.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.291.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Guinther, P. M. (2018). Contextual influence over deriving another’s false beliefs using a relational triangulation perspective taking protocol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110, 500–521.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.480.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Hahs, A. D. (2015). Teaching prerequisite perspective-taking skills to children with autism. International Journal of Psychology & Behavioral Sciences, 5, 115–120.  https://doi.org/10.5923/j.ijpbs.20150503.02.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behavioral & Brain Science, 21, 803–865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Happé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00909.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Harrington, L., Siegert, R. J., & McClure, J. (2005). Theory of mind in schizophrenia: A critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10, 249–286.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800444000056.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Harwood, M. D., & Farrar, M. J. (2006). Conflicting emotions: The connection between affective perspective taking and theory of mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 401–418.  https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X50302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Hayes, J., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2016). Assessing and training young children in same and different relations using the relational evaluation procedure (REP). The Psychological Record, 66, 547–561.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0191-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Hayes, S. C. (1984). Making sense of spirituality. Behaviorism, 12, 99–110.Google Scholar
  90. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
  91. Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1993). Some applied implications of a contemporary behavior-analytic account of verbal events. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 283–301.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392637.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  92. Heagle, A. I., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2006). Teaching perspective-taking skills to typically developing children through derived relational responding. Journal of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention, 3, 1–34.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Hendriks, A. L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., McEnteggart, C., De Mey, H. R. A., Witteman, C. J. M., Janssen, G. T. L., & Egger, J. I. M. (2016). The relationship between theory of mind and relational frame theory: Convergence of perspective-taking measures. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 13, 17–23.Google Scholar
  94. Hoffman, S. G., Doan, S. N., Sprung, M., Wilson, A., Ebesutani, C., Andrews, L. A., et al. (2016). Training children’s theory-of-mind: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition, 150, 200–212.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children with autism to mind-read: A practical guide for teachers and parents. London, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
  96. Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590–619.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2014). Associative concept learning, stimulus equivalence, and relational frame theory: Working out the similarities and differences between human and non-human behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 156–160.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.60.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Hyland, J. M., Smyth, S., O’Hora, D., & Leslie, J. C. (2014). The effect of before and after instructions on the speed of sequential responding. The Psychological Record, 64, 311–319.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0026-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Idson, L. C., Chugh, D., Bereby-Meyer, Y., Moran, S., Grosskopf, B., & Bazerman, M. (2004). Overcoming focusing failures in competitive environments. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 159–172.  https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Im-Bolter, N., Agostino, A., & Owens-Jaffray, K. (2016). Theory of mind in middle childhood and early adolescence: Different from before? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 98–115.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.12.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Izard, C. E., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B. P., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18–23.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00304.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Jackson, M. L., Mendoza, D. R., & Adams, A. N. (2014). Teaching a deictic relational repertoire to children with autism. The Psychological Record, 64, 791–802.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0078-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Janssen, G., De May, H., Hendriks, A., Koppers, A., Kaarsemaker, M., Witteman, C., & Egger, J. (2014). Assessing deictic relational responding in individuals with social anxiety disorder: Evidence of perspective-taking difficulties. The Psychological Record, 64, 21–29.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0013-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Kavanagh, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, P. M. D., McEnteggart, C., & Finn, M. (2018). Exploring differential trial-type effects and the impact of a read-aloud procedure on deictic relational responding on the IRAP. The Psychological Record, 68, 163–176.  https://doi.org/10.1007/S40732-018-0276-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Kavanagh, D., Hussey, I., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Using the IRAP to explore natural language statements. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 5, 247–251.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2016.10.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Kavanagh, D., Roelandt, A., Van Raemdonck, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2019). The on-going search for perspective-taking IRAPs: Exploring the potential of the natural language IRAP. The Psychological Record, 69(2), 291–314.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-019-00333-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Kent, G., Galvin, E., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2017). Relational responding: Testing, training and sequencing effects among children with autism and typically developing children. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 22, 94–110.  https://doi.org/10.1037/bdb0000041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Keysar, B., & Barr, D. J. (2002). Self anchoring in conversation: why language users do not do what they should. In T. Gilovich, D. W. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement (pp. 150–166). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition, 89, 25–41.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00064-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1207–1219.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024808.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Lattal, K. A. (1975). Reinforcement contingencies as discriminative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 23, 241–246.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1975.23-241.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  112. LeBlanc, L. A., Coates, A. M., Daneshvar, S., Charlop-Christy, M. H., Morrus, C., & Lancaster, B. M. (2003). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 253–257.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-253.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  113. Lipkens, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 201–239.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  114. Lovett, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2014). An evaluation of multiple exemplar instruction to teach perspective-taking skills to adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19, 22–36.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1998.69-327.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  116. Luciano, C., Gómez-Becerra, I., & Rodríguez Valverde, M. (2007). The role of multiple-exemplar training and naming in establishing derived equivalence in an infant. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 349–365.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2007.08-06.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  117. Luo, Y., & Baillargeon, R. (2007). Do 12.5-month-old infants consider what object others can see when interpreting their action? Cognition, 105, 489–512.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Macdonald, R., Anderson, J., Dube, W. V., Geckler, A., Green, G., Holcomb, W., et al. (2006). Behavioral assessment of joint attention: A methodological report. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 138–150.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2004.09.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. McGreal, C., Hyland, J., O’Hora, D., & Hogan, M. (2016). Mutual entailment of temporal relations in younger and older adults: Reversing order judgements. The Psychological Record, 66, 419–428.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0182-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Perspective-taking as relational responding: A developmental profile. The Psychological Record, 54, 115–144.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2006). Understanding false belief as generalized operant behavior. The Psychological Record, 56, 341–364.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., Stewart, I., & Dymond, S. (2007). Deictic relational complexity and the development of deception. The Psychological Record, 57, 517–531.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. McKinnon, M. C., & Moscovitch, M. (2007). Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition, 102, 179–218.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.011.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  125. Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82, 661–673.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01571.x.
  126. Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2001). Perspective-taking and its foundation in joint attention. In N. Eilan, H. Lerman, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Perception, causation, and objectivity. Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 286–304). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  127. Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 60–61.  https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X55370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Montag, C., Dziobek, I., Richter, I. S., Neuhaus, K., Lehmann, A., Rudolf, S., et al. (2011). Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: Evidence from video-based assessment. Psychiatry Research, 186, 203–209.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Montoya-Rodríguez, M. M., & Molina Cobos, F. J. (2018). Assessing perspective-taking in children through different formats of deictic framing protocol. In H. T. Hou & C. S. Ryan, (Eds.), Behavior analysis, IntechOpen. doi: https://doi.org/10.5772/intechopne.74539.
  130. Montoya-Rodríguez, M. M., Molina, F. J., & McHugh, L. (2017). A review of relational frame theory research into deictic relational responding. The Psychological Record, 67, 569–579.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0216-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2017). Investigating relational framing of categorization in young children. The Psychological Record, 67, 519–536.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0255-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2018). Facilitating relational framing of classification in young children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 8, 55–68.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.04.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Murphy, C., Lyons, K., Kelly, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2019). Using the Teacher IRAP (T-IRAP) interactive computerized program to teach complex flexible relational responding to children with diagnosed autism spectrum disorder. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 12, 52–65.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-018-00302-9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  134. Németh, N., Mátrai, P., Hegyi, P., Czéh, B., Czopf, L., Hussain, A., et al. (2018). Theory of mind disturbances in borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 270, 143–153.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.049.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  135. Novak, G. (1996). Developmental psychology: Dynamical systems and behavior analysis. Reno, NV: Context Press.Google Scholar
  136. Novak, G. (1998). A behavioral systems theory of development. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 24, 181–196.Google Scholar
  137. Novak, G., & Pelaez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development: A behavioural systems approach. London, UK: Sage.Google Scholar
  138. O’Connor, J., Rafferty, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). The role of verbal behavior, stimulus nameability, and familiarity on the equivalence performances of autistic and normally developing children. The Psychological Record, 59, 53–74.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. O'Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Derived relational networks and control by novel instructions: A possible model of generative verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54, 437–460.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. O’Hora, D., Peláez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: Correlating relational performance with performance on the Wais-III. The Psychological Record, 58, 569–584.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Okuda, K., & Inoue, M. (2000). A behavior analytic view of teaching “theory of mind” to children with autism: Stimulus control and generalization on false belief tasks. Japanese Psychological Review, 43, 427–442.Google Scholar
  142. O’Neill, J., & Weil, T. M. (2014). Training deictic relational responding in people diagnosed with schizophrenia. The Psychological Record, 64, 301–310.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0005-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. O'Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three chronometric indices of relational responding as predictors of performances on a brief intelligence test: The importance of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59, 119–132.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Peláez, M. (2009). Joint attention and social referencing in infancy as precursors of derived relational responding. In R. A. Rehfeldt & Y. Barnes-Holmes (Eds.), Derived relational responding applications for learners with autism and other developmental disabilities: A progressive guide to change (pp. 63–78). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.Google Scholar
  145. Peláez, M., Virues-Ortega, J., & Gewritz, J. L. (2012). Acquisition of social referencing via discrimination training in infants. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 23–36.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2012.45-23.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  146. Peláez-Nogueras, M., & Gewirtz, J. (1997). The context of stimulus control in behavior analysis. In D. M. Baer & E. M. Pinkston (Eds.), Environment and behavior (pp. 30–42). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
  147. Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. M., & Leekman, S. R. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child’s theory of mind: Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Development, 60, 689–700.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory of mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, 502–517.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  149. Piaget, J. (1948). The moral judgment of the child. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
  150. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
  151. Pickup, G. J. (2008). Relationship between theory of mind and executive function in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Psychopathology, 41, 206–213.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000125554.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  152. Pickup, G. J., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia: Symptomatology, severity and specificity. Psychological Medicine, 31, 207–220.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003385.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Preißler, S., Dziobek, I., Ritter, K., Heekeren, H. R., & Roepke, S. (2010). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder: Evidence for disturbed recognition of the emotions, thoughts, and intentions of others. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience., 4, 182.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00182.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  154. Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Rehfeldt, R. A., Dillen, J. E., Ziomek, M. M., & Kowalchuk, R. K. (2007). Assessing relational learning deficits in perspective-taking in children with high-functional autism spectrum disorder. The Psychological Record, 57, 23–47.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1996). Arbitrarily applicable relational responding and sexual categorization: A critical test of the derived difference relation. The Psychological Record, 46, 451–475.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Roepke, S., Vater, A., Preißler, S., Heekeren, H. R., & Dziobek, I. (2013). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 6, 195.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00195.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  158. Savla, G. N., Vella, L., Armstrong, C. C., Penn, D. L., & Twamley, E. W. (2013). Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39, 979–992.  https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs080.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  159. Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 54, 628–635.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Scherzer, P., Leveille, E., Achim, A., Boisseau, E., & Stip, E. (2012). A study of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–11.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Schick, B., de Villers, P., de Villers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78, 376–396.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Schlinger, H. D. (2009). Theory of mind: An overview and behavioral perspective. The Psychological Record, 59, 435–448.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Shur, S., Barcai-Goodman, L., Medlovich, S., Harari, H., & Levkovitz, Y. (2007). Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 149, 11–23.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.10.018.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  164. Sharp, C., Pane, H., Ha, C., Venta, A., Patel, A. B., Sturek, J., & Fonagy, P. (2011). Theory of mind and emotion regulation difficulties in adolescents with borderline traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 563–573.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  165. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 14, 5–13.  https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1401.05.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Sigman, M., & Capps, L. (1997). Children with autism: A developmental perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  167. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
  168. Slattery, B., & Stewart, I. (2014). Hierarchical classification as relational framing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 101, 61–75.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.63.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  169. Slaughter, V., & McConnell, D. (2003). Emergence of joint attention: Relationships between gaze following, social referencing, imitation, and naming in infancy. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 54–71.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00221320309597503.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  170. Sodian, B., & Kristen-Antonow, S. (2015). Declarative joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1190–1200.  https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000039.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  171. Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Metz, U. (2007). Now I see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Developmental Science., 10, 199–204.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00580.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  172. Spradlin, J. E., & Brady, N. (2008). A behaviour analytic interpretation of theory of mind. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 8, 335–350.Google Scholar
  173. Steele, D., & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.  https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1991.56-519.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  174. Sullivan, K., Zaitchik, D., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994). Preschoolers can attribute second-order beliefs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 395–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Evaluating the Theory-of-Mind hypothesis of autism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 311–315.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00527.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  176. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 32, 1123–1134. https://doi.org/10.1469-7610.1991.tb00353.x.Google Scholar
  177. Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124–129.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.008.
  178. Uekermann, J., Kramer, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Schimmelmann, B. G., Hebebrand, J., Daum, I., et al. (2010). Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 734–743.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  179. Vahey, N. A., Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). A meta-analysis of criterion effects for the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP) in the clinical domain. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 59–65.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  180. Vaskin, A., Antonsen, B. T., Fretland, R. A., Dziobek, I., Sundet, K., & Wilberg, T. (2015). Theory of mind in women with borderline personality disorder or schizophrenia: differences in overall ability and error patterns. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1239.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  181. Vescio, T. K., Sechrist, G. B., & Paolucci, M. P. (2003). Perspective taking and prejudice reduction: The mediational role of empathy arousal and situational attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 455–472.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  182. Vilardaga, R., Estévez, A., Levin, M. E., & Hayes, S. (2012). Deictic relational responding, empathy, and experiential avoidance as predictors of social anhedonia: Further contributions from relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 62, 409–432.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  183. Villatte, M., Monestés, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E. F., & Loas, G. (2008). Assessing deictic relational responding in social anhedonia: A functional approach to the development of theory of mind impairments. Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 4, 360–373.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  184. Villatte, M., Monestés, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E. F., & Loas, G. (2010a). Assessing perspective taking in schizophrenia using relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 60, 413–436.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Villatte, M., Monestés, J. L., McHugh, L., Freixa i Baqué, E. F., & Loas, G. (2010b). Adopting the perspective of another in belief attribution: Contribution of relational fame theory to the understanding of impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 125–134.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  186. Vitale, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Campbell, C. (2008). Facilitating responding in accordance with the relational frame of comparison; systematic empirical analyses. The Psychological Record, 58, 365–390.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  187. Weil, T. M., Hayes, S. C., & Capurro, P. (2011). Establishing a deictic relational repertoire in young children. The Psychological Record, 61, 371–390.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  188. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1009-3920/2001/7203-0001.Google Scholar
  189. Wellman, H. M., Phillips, A. T., & Rodriguez, T. (2000). Young children’s understanding of perception, desire, and emotion. Child Development, 71, 898–912. https://doi.org/10.1009-3920/2000/7104-0011.Google Scholar
  190. Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behaviour modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 44, 456–468.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  191. Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82, 177–195. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2004.82-177.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association for Behavior Analysis International 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Experimental, Clinical, and Health PsychologyGhent UniversityGhentBelgium

Personalised recommendations