What is your discount rate? Experimental evidence of foresters’ risk and time preferences

Original Paper
Part of the following topical collections:
  1. Risk Analysis

Abstract

• Key message

The elicited time preference rate of German foresters is around 4.1%. Foresters working for private enterprises are more risk-averse and have a lower time preference than other foresters. This group difference should be taken into account for modeling and policy making.

• Context

Due to very long rotations, forestry investment calculations heavily depend on the underlying discount rate. There is an ongoing debate about the appropriate discount rate to apply in forestry, particularly in light of concerns regarding inter-generational justice, forest risks, and the provision of future positive externalities from forestry. For sound policy making however, knowledge is lacking on the risk and time preferences of foresters.

• Aims

The present study aims to provide detailed information about risk and time preferences as essential aspects of the discounting behavior.

• Methods

Therefore, we conducted an economic experiment with 142 German foresters. Both risk and time preferences affect discounting behavior, which is why they are estimated jointly but analyzed specifically.

• Results

Participating foresters’ risk attitudes range between risk-neutral and very risk-averse, where the sample can be mostly characterized as risk-averse. Time preference discount rates range mostly between 0 and 7%, with 4.1% as a central value. These results are group-specific: foresters working for a private forest enterprise are more risk-averse and have a lower discount rate than other participating foresters.

• Conclusion

Foresters’ time preferences exceed the usual rates of return in German forestry, which might be explained by additional utility attributed to forest amenity values or the risk-decreasing effects of forest assets in their portfolio.

Keywords

Time preference Risk attitude Foresters Economic experiment Maximum likelihood estimation Policy support 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Bernhard Möhring for constructive discussions and two anonymous referees and the editors for helpful comments and suggestions.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

13595_2017_683_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (465 kb)
(PDF 464 KB)

References

  1. Anderhub V, Guth W, Gneezy U, Sonsino D (2001) On the interaction of risk and time preferences: an experimental study. Ger Econ Rev 2(3):239–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andersen S, Harrison GW, Lau MI, Rutström EE (2008) Eliciting risk and time preferences. Econometrica 76(3):583–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson J, Burks SV, Carpenter J, Götte L, Maurer K, Nosenzo D, Potter R, Rocha K, Rustichini A (2013) Self-selection and variations in the laboratory measurement of other-regarding preferences across subject pools: evidence from one college student and two adult samples. Exp Econ 16(2):170–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson LR, Freeborn BA, Hulbert JP (2012) Risk aversion and tacit collusion in a bertrand duopoly experiment. Rev Ind Organ 40(1):37–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson LR, Stafford SL (2009) Individual decision-making experiments with risk and intertemporal choice. J Risk Uncertain 38(1):51–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Andreoni J, Sprenger C (2012) Risk preferences are not time preferences. Am Econ Rev 102(7):3357–3376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Atmadja SS, Sills EO (2013) Forest management and landowners’ discount rates in the Southern United States. In: Kant S (ed) Post-Faustmann forest resource economics, sustainability, economics, and natural resources. Springer, Dordrecht, Netherlands, pp 91–123Google Scholar
  8. Binmore K (1999) Why experiment in economics?. Econ J 109(453):16–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Botzen WJW, van den Bergh JCJM (2014) Specifications of social welfare in economic studies of climate policy: overview of criteria and related policy insights. Environ Resour Econ 58(1):1–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brukas V, Jellesmark Thorsen B, Helles F, Tarp P (2001) Discount rate and harvest policy: implications for Baltic forestry. For Policy Econ 2(2):143–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brunette M, Foncel J, Kéré EN (2014) Attitude towards risk and production decision: an empirical analysis on French private forest owners. Centre d’études et de recherches internationales working paper no 2014-10, Montréal, FranceGoogle Scholar
  12. Bullard S, Gunter JE (2002) Discount rates for nonindustrial private forest landowners in Mississippi: how high a hurdle?. South J Appl For 26(1):26–31Google Scholar
  13. Coble KH, Lusk JL (2010) At the nexus of risk and time preferences: an experimental investigation. J Risk Uncertain 41(1):67–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coller M, Williams MB (1999) Eliciting individual discount rates. Exp Econ 2(2):107–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Domínguez G, Shannon M (2011) A wish, a fear and a complaint: understanding the (dis)engagement of forest owners in forest management. Eur J For Res 130(3):435–450CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Duquette E, Higgins N, Horowitz J (2014) Inferring discount rates from time-preference experiments. Econ Lett 123(2):212– 215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Exadaktylos F, Espín AM, Branas-Garza P (2013) Experimental subjects are not different. Sci Rep (3):1213Google Scholar
  18. Eyvindson K, Kangas A (2016) Integrating risk preferences in forest harvest scheduling. Ann For Sci 73 (2):321–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. FAO (2006) Time for action: changing the gender situation in forestry: Report of the team of specialists on gender and forestry. Food and Agricultural Organization of the United Nations (FAO), RomeGoogle Scholar
  20. Federal Statistical Office of Germany (2013a) Aufwendungen privater haushalte für den privaten konsum. https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Thematisch/EinkommenKonsumLebensbedingungen/Konsumausgaben/EVS_AufwendungprivaterHaushalte.html
  21. Federal Statistical Office of Germany (2013b) Erwerbstätigkeit in Deutschland - Ergebnisse des Zensus 2011. https://www.destatis.de/DE/Publikationen/Wirtschaftstatistik/Zensus/Erwerbstaetigkeit2011_92014.html
  22. Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’donoghue T (2002) Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J Econ Lit 40(2):351–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gerst JM (2015) Der Grenzzins als Entscheidungskriterium in der Forstwirtschaft: Methodisches Konzept und Empirische Evidenz, vol. 21 of Schriften zur Forst- und Umweltökonomie. Sauerländer Verlag, Bad OrbGoogle Scholar
  24. Granello DH, Wheaton JE (2004) Online data collection: strategies for research. J Couns Dev 82(4):387–393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Harrison GW, Lau MI, Rutström EE (2007) Estimating risk attitudes in Denmark: a field experiment. Scand J Econ 109(2):341–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Harrison GW, Rutström EE (2008) Risk aversion in the laboratory. In: Cox JC, Harrison GW (eds) Risk aversion in experiments, research in experimental economics, Emerald Group Publishing Limited. Bingley, Great Britain, pp 41–196Google Scholar
  27. Hepburn C, Duncan S, Papachristodoulou A (2010) Behavioural economics, hyperbolic discounting and environmental policy. Environ Resour Econ 46(2):189–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hepburn CJ, Koundouri P (2007) Recent advances in discounting: implications for forest economics. J For Econ 13(2-3):169–189Google Scholar
  29. Hermann D, Musshoff O (2015) Anchoring effects in experimental discount rate elicitation. Appl Econ Lett 23(14):1022–1025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Holt CA, Laury SK (2002) Risk aversion and incentive effects. Am Econ Rev 92(5):1644–1655CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hyytiäinen K, Penttinen M (2008) Applying portfolio optimisation to the harvesting decisions of non-industrial private forest owners. For Policy Econ 10(3):151–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kable JW, Glimcher PW (2010) An ’as soon as possible’ effect in human intertemporal decision making: behavioral evidence and neural mechanisms. J Neurophys 103(5):2513–2531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kant S (1999) Endogenous rate of time preference, traditional communities, and sustainable forest management. J Soc Econ Dev 2(1):65–87Google Scholar
  34. Kant S, Wang S, Deegen P, Hostettler M, von Detten R, Howard T, Laband D, Montgomery C, Robert N, Sekot W, Valatin G, Zhang D (2013) New frontiers of forest economics. For Policy Econ 35:1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kronrad GD, de Steiguer JE (1983) Relationships between discount rates and investment lengths among nonindustrial private landowners: small woodlot r&d program. Research Note Series (19)Google Scholar
  36. Lundgren T (2005) Assessing the investment performance of swedish timberland: a capital asset pricing model approach. Land Econ 81(3):353–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Möhring B (2014) Gibt es einen richtigen Zinssatz bei der forstlichen Wertermittlung?. Wertermittlungsforum 32(2):72–78Google Scholar
  38. Musshoff O, Maart-Noelck SC (2014) An experimental analysis of the behavior of forestry decision-makers – the example of timing in sales decisions. For Policy Econ 41:31–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. OECD (1999) Classifying educational programmes: manual for ISCED-97 implementation in OECD countries. Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) Publishing, ParisGoogle Scholar
  40. Prestemon JP, Wear DN (2000) Linking harvest choices to timber supply. For Sci 46(3):377–389Google Scholar
  41. Price C (1993) Time, discounting, and value. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  42. Price C (2011) Optimal rotation with declining discount rate. J For Econ 17(3):307–318Google Scholar
  43. Price C (2014) Temporal aspects in forest economics. In: Kant S, Alavalapati JRR (eds) Handbook of forest resource economics, Earthscan from Routledge. London, Great Britain, pp 50–66Google Scholar
  44. Roe BE (2015) The risk attitudes of U.S. farmers. Appl Econ Perspect Policy 37(4):553–574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Samuelson PA (1976) Economics of forestry in an evolving society. Econ Inq 14(4):466–492CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sauter P, Hermann D, Musshoff O (2016) Risk attitudes of farmers, foresters and students: an experimental multimethod comparison. In: Australian agricultural and resource economics society (ed.), AARES 2016 annual conferenceGoogle Scholar
  47. Simonsen R (2013) Optimal regeneration method – planting vs. natural regeneration of scots pine in Northern Sweden. Silva Fennica 47(2):1–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© INRA and Springer-Verlag France SAS, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department for Agricultural Economics and Rural DevelopmentGeorg-August-Universität GöttingenGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations