Advertisement

Review of Philosophy and Psychology

, Volume 10, Issue 2, pp 337–362 | Cite as

Emotional Awareness and Responsible Agency

  • Nathan StoutEmail author
Article

Abstract

This paper aims to further examine the relationship between self-awareness and agency by focusing on the role that emotional awareness plays in prominent conceptions of responsibility. One promising way of approaching this task is by focusing on individuals who display impairments in emotional awareness and then examining the effects (if any) that these impairments have on their apparent responsibility for the actions that they perform. Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) as well as other clinical groups who evince high degrees of the personality construct known as alexithymia, I will argue, do, in fact, display impairments in emotional self-awareness, and, so, may provide some insight into the relationship between this awareness and the capacity for responsible agency. More specifically, individuals with ASD may provide us with evidence that robust emotional self-awareness is not a necessary condition for responsible agency or that lacking such a capacity could undermine some otherwise plausible sufficient conditions for responsibility. The aim of the paper, then, is twofold. First, it will aim to show that ASD ought to be understood, in part, as a disorder of emotional self-awareness. Section 2 presents evidence relating to the emotional profile characteristic of individuals with ASD and argues that the most striking feature of this profile is the way in which the individual seems to be separated from his or her emotions in an important sense. Second, the paper aims to show that this distinctive feature of the emotions in ASD casts serious doubt on some prominent accounts of moral responsibility. To this end, section 3.1 presents a challenge to some widely accepted “subjective conditions” for responsibility, namely those articulated by John Martin Fischer and Ravizza (1998) and makes a case, based on the empirical data regarding autism, that these conditions actually are not necessary for one’s being a responsible agent. Section 3.2 then presents a challenge for theories of responsible agency which assign primary importance to the connection between an agent’s actions and her judgment-sensitive attitudes. I argue there that the evidence from autism suggests that these theories fail in their efforts to provide sufficient conditions for responsibility.

References

  1. Accardo, Pasquale J., and Barbara Y. Whitman. 2002. Dictionary of developmental disabilities terminology. 2nd ed. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co..Google Scholar
  2. American Psychiatric Association. n.d. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Fifth Edition. Google Scholar
  3. Atwood, Anthony, Uta Frith, and Beate Hermelin. 1988. The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and down’s syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 18 (2): 241–257.Google Scholar
  4. Baron-Cohen, S., A. Leslie, and U. Frith. 1985. Does the autistic child have a 'theory of mind'? Cognition 21: 37–46.Google Scholar
  5. Baron-Cohen, S., M. O’Riordan, V. Stone, R. Jones, and K. Plaisted. 1999. Recognition of Faux Pas by normally developing children and children with Asperger Syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29 (5): 407–418.Google Scholar
  6. Beadle, Janelle N., Sergio Paradiso, Alexandria Salerno, and Laurie M. McCormick. 2013. Alexithymia, emotional empathy, and self-regulation in anorexia nervosa. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 25 (2): 107–120.Google Scholar
  7. Begeer, Sander, Hans M. Koot, Carolien Rieffe, Mark Meerum Terwogt, and Hedy Stegge. 2008. Emotional competence in children with autism: Diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence. Developmental Review 28: 342–369.Google Scholar
  8. Belcher, Hannah Louise. 2015. Watch: Women Diagnosed with Autism Late Tell Their Stories. Autism Speaks. Web. Accessed 25 Feb 2016. https://www.autismspeaks.org/blog/2015/09/17/watch-women-diagnosed-autism-late-tell-their-stories
  9. Berna, Fabrice, Anja S. Göritz, Johanna Schröder, Romain Coutelle, Jean-Marie Danion, Christine V. Cuervo-Lombard, and Steffen Moritz. 2016. Self-disorders in individuals with autistic traits: Contribution of reduced autobiographical reasoning capacities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46: 2587–2598.Google Scholar
  10. Bird, Geoffrey and Richard Cook. 2013. Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry. No. 3:1–8.Google Scholar
  11. Bird, Geoffrey, Clare Press, Daniel C. Richardson. 2011. The role of alexithymia in reduced eye-fixation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41: 1556–1564.Google Scholar
  12. Blair, R. James. 1996. Brief report: Morality in the autistic child. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 26: 571–579.Google Scholar
  13. Boryz, S., H. Spitz, and B. Dorans. 1982. Tower of Hanoi performance of retarded young adults and nonretarded children as a function of solution length and goal state. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33: 87–110.Google Scholar
  14. Brundson, V., and F. Happé. 2014. Exploring the ‘fractionation’ of autism at the cognitive level. Autism 18 (1): 17–30.Google Scholar
  15. Capps, Lisa, Connie Kasari, Nurit Yirmiya, and Marian Sigman. 1993. Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 61 (3): 475–484.Google Scholar
  16. Cook, Richard, Rebecca Brewer, Punit Shah, and Geoffrey Bird. 2013. Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychological Science 24 (5): 723–732.Google Scholar
  17. Dawson, Geraldine, Deborah Hill, Art Spencer, Larry Galpert, and Linda Watson. 1990. Affective exchanges between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 18 (3): 335–345.Google Scholar
  18. Fischer, John Martin. 2006. The free will revolution (continued). The Journal of Ethics 10 (3): 315–345.Google Scholar
  19. Fischer, John Martin, and Mark Ravizza. 1998. Responsibility and control: A theory of moral responsibility. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Frankfurt, Harry G. 1998. “Freedom of the Will and the Concept of a Person.” 1971. In The importance of what we care about, 11–25. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Frith, U. 1989. Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  22. Frith, U., and F. Happé. 1994. Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition 50: 115–132.Google Scholar
  23. Frith, U., J. Morton, and A. Leslie. 1991. The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Trends in Neurosciences 14 (10): 433–438.Google Scholar
  24. Golan, Ofer, Emma Ashwin, Yael Granader, Suzy McClintock, Kate Day, Victoria Leggett, and Simon Baron-Cohen. 2010. Enhancing emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum conditions: An intervention using animated vehicles with real emotional faces. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40: 269–279.Google Scholar
  25. Grant, D., and E. Berg. 1948. A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigle-type card sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology 32: 404–411.Google Scholar
  26. Happé, F., and A. Ronald. 2008. The “fractionable autism triad”: A review of evidence from behavioral, genetic, cognitive, and neural research. Neuropsychology Review 18 (4): 287–304.Google Scholar
  27. Hill, E. 2004. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review 24: 189–233.Google Scholar
  28. Hill, Elisabeth, Sylvie Berthoz, and Uta Frith. 2004. Brief report: Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and their relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 229–235.Google Scholar
  29. Hobson, R. Peter. 2014. The coherence of autism. Autism 18 (1): 6–16.Google Scholar
  30. Hobson, R. Peter, Gayathri Chidambi, Anthony Lee, Meyer Jessica, U. Muller, J.I.M. Carpendale, M. Bibok, and T.P. Racine. 2006. Foundations for self-awareness: An exploration through autism. Monographs of the Society for Reasearch in Child Development. 71 (2): 1–166.Google Scholar
  31. Hopkins, Ingrid Maria, Michael W. Gower, Trista A. Perez, Dana S. Smith, Franklin R. Amthor, F. Casey Wimsatt, and Fred J. Biasini. 2011. Avatar assistant: Improving social skills in students with an ASD through a computer-based intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41: 1543–1555.Google Scholar
  32. Kanner, Leo. 1943. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. The Nervous Child 2: 217–250.Google Scholar
  33. Kennedy, Daniel P., and Ralph Adolphs. 2012. Perception of emotions from facial expressions in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia 50: 3313–3319.Google Scholar
  34. Kenworthy, L., B. Yerys, L. Gutermuth Anthony, and G. Wallace. 2008. Understanding executive control in autism spectrum disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychological Review 18: 320–338.Google Scholar
  35. Legrand, Dorothée. 2006. The bodily self: The sensori-motor roots of pre-reflective self-consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 5: 89–118.Google Scholar
  36. Leslie, Alan M., Ron Mallon, and Jennifer A. DiCorcia. 2006. Transgressors, victims, and cry babies: Is basic moral judgment spared in autism? Social Neuroscience 1: 270–283.Google Scholar
  37. Losh, Molly, and Lisa Capps. 2006. Understanding of emotional experience in autism: Insights from the personal accounts of high-functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology 42 (5): 809–818.Google Scholar
  38. Markram, Kamila, and Henry Markram. 2010. The intense world theory – A unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 4 (224): 1–29.Google Scholar
  39. Markram, Henry, Tania Rinaldi, and Kamila Markram. 2007. The intense world syndrome – An alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience 1 (1): 77–96.Google Scholar
  40. McGeer, Victoria. 2008. The varieties of moral agency: Lessons from autism (and psychopathy). In Moral psychology volume 3. The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development, ed. Walter Sinnott-Armstrong, 227–257. Massachussetts: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  41. McKenna, Michael. 2012. Conversation and responsibility. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Mele, Alfred. 2006. Fischer and Ravizza on moral responsibility. The Journal of Ethics 10 (3): 283–294.Google Scholar
  43. Montebarocci, Ornella, Paola Surcinelli, Nicola Rossi, and Bruno Baldaro. 2011. Alexithymia, verbal ability and emotion recognition. Psychiatric Quarterly 82: 245–252.Google Scholar
  44. Moran, J., L. Young, R. Saxe, S. Lee, D. O'Young, P. Mavros, and J. Gabrieli. 2011. Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (7): 2688–2692.Google Scholar
  45. Morin, Alain. 2011. Self-awareness part 1: Definition, measures, effects, functions, and antecedents. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 5 (10): 807–823.Google Scholar
  46. Nuske, Heather J., Giacomo Vivanti, and Cheryl Dissanayake. 2013. Are emotion impairments unique to, universal, or specific in autism spectrum disorder? A comprehensive review. Cognition and Emotion. 27 (6): 1042–1061.Google Scholar
  47. Ozonoff, S., B. Pennington, and S. Rogers. 1991. Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 32 (7): 1081–1105.Google Scholar
  48. Pellicano, E. 2007. Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: Clues to developmental primacy. Developmental Psychology 43 (4): 974–990.Google Scholar
  49. Rajendran, G., and P. Mitchell. 2007. Cognitive theories of autism. Developmental Review 27: 224–260.Google Scholar
  50. Rieffe, Carolien, Mark Meerum Terwogt, and Katerina Kotronopoulou. 2007. Awareness of single and multiple emotions in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37: 455–465.Google Scholar
  51. Russell, Paul. 2004. Responsibility and the condition of moral sense. Philosophical Topics 32 (1 & 2): 287–305.Google Scholar
  52. Scanlon, T.M. 1998. What we owe to each other. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Shoemaker, David. 2011. Attributability, answerability, and accountability: toward a wider theory of moral responsibility. Ethics 121 (3): 602–632.Google Scholar
  54. Smith, Angela. 2005. Responsibility for attitudes: activity and passivity in mental life. Ethics 115 (2): 236–271.Google Scholar
  55. Smith, Angela. 2008. Control, responsibility, and moral assessment. Philosophical Studies 138: 367–392.Google Scholar
  56. Smith, Angela. 2015. Responsibility as answerability. Inquiry 58 (2): 99–126.Google Scholar
  57. Stout, Nathan. 2016a. Autism, episodic memory, and moral exemplars. Philosophical Psychology 29: 858–870.Google Scholar
  58. Stout, Nathan. 2016b. Moral agency and responsibility: lessons from autism spectrum disorder. Unpublished Dissertation. Tulane University.Google Scholar
  59. Stout, Nathan. 2016c. Reasons-responsiveness and moral responsibility: the case of autism. The Journal of Ethics 20: 401–418.Google Scholar
  60. Taylor, Graeme. 2000. Recent developments in alexithymia theory and research. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 45 (2): 134–142.Google Scholar
  61. Uljarevic, Mirko, and Antonia Hamilton. 2013. Recognition of emotions in autism: A formal meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43: 1517–1526.Google Scholar
  62. Wimmer, H., and J. Perner. 1983. Beliefs about beliefs – representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young childrens understanding of deception. Cognition 13 (1): 103–128.Google Scholar
  63. Wing, L., and J. Gould. 1979. Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 9: 11–29.Google Scholar
  64. Wood, Roger L.L., and Claire Williams. 2008. Inability to empathize following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 14: 289–296.Google Scholar
  65. Yirmiya, Nurit, Marian D. Sigman, Connie Kasari, and Peter Mundy. 1992. Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development 63: 150–160.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PhilosophyTulane UniversityNew OrleansUSA

Personalised recommendations