Emotional expressions with minimal facial muscle actions. Report 1: Cues and targets
- 13 Downloads
Abstract
We tested the hypothesis that a still-face person keeping direct gaze can express an emotion. The prospective study involved healthy volunteers as senders (n = 100). Four pictures of the face of a participant who tried to express certain emotion by gaze were obtained (Neutral – Angry – Sad – Happy). Twenty blinded judges judged the ability to express the emotions. We used the pupil/canthal tilt index for analysis of pupil dilatation. The pictures with emotions (n = 300) presented perfectly expressed emotions – 207 (69%), flawed expressions – 38 (12.66%), and lack of expression or inability to express an emotion – 55 (18.33%). The pupil/tilt index was: neutral gaze – 0.18, angry gaze – 0.21, sad gaze – 0.2, and happy gaze – 0.19. Majority of senders express an emotion by gaze very good. The facial expression of an emotion can be adequately achieved with only minimal muscular involvement when the gaze is the main component of such expression. An “emotional gaze” during a still-face or static face situation may be a result of the combined slight activity of small intraorbital muscles and broader involvement of sympathetic and parasympathetic reactions.
Keywords
Emotions Gaze Facial expressions Adolescents Pupil dilatationNotes
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Mordechai Cohen, Orit Rome, Leor Sinai, Rachelle Sevitt, and Alex Lasky for assistance in the research in Israel, and Roger Kassebaum, Damon Scoville, Pauline Kim, and Daniel Bareket for assistance in the research in the USA.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest
Yulia Roitblat, Sabrina Cohensedgh, Eden Frig-Levinson, Ethan Suman, and Michael Shterenshis declare no potential conflict of interest.
References
- Aïte, A., Cassotti, M., Linzarini, A., Osmont, A., Houdé, O., & Borst, G. (2018). Adolescents' inhibitory control: Keep it cool or lose control. Developmental Science, 21(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12491.
- Allen, H. (1875). Studies in the facial region (p. 3). Philadelphia: Lippincott & Co..Google Scholar
- Bänziger, T., Hosoya, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2015). Path models of vocal emotion communication. PLoS One, 10(9), e0136675. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136675.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bayless, S. J., Glover, M., Taylor, M. J., & Itier, R. J. (2011). Is it in the eyes? Dissociating the role of emotion and perceptual features of emotionally expressive faces in modulating orienting to eye gaze. Visual Cognition, 19(4), 483–510.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Berenbaum, H., & Oltmanns, T. F. (1992). Emotional experience and expression in schizophrenia and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(1), 37–44.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brunswik, E. (1939). The conceptual focus of some psychological systems. Journal of Unified Science (Erkenntnis), 8, 36–49.Google Scholar
- Bush, L. K., Barr, C. L., McHugo, G. J., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1989). The effects of facial control and facial mimicry on subjective reactions to comedy routines. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 31–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Chaplin, T. M., Klein, M. R., Cole, P. M., & Turpyn, C. C. (2017). Developmental change in emotion expression in frustrating situations: The roles of context and gender. Infant and Child Development, 26(6), e2028. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2028.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- DiGirolamo, M. A., & Russell, J. A. (2017). The emotion seen in a face can be a methodological artifact: The process of elimination hypothesis. Emotion, 17(3), 538–546.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dijk, C., Fischer, A. H., Morina, N., van Eeuwijk, C., & van Kleef, G. A. (2018). Effects of social anxiety on emotional mimicry and contagion: Feeling negative, but smiling politely. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42(1), 81–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0266-z.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
- Fridlund, A. J. (1998). The new ethology of human facial expressions. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (p. 112). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Frijda, N. H., & Tcherkassof, A. (1998). Facial expressions as modes of action readiness. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (p. 94). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Gifford, R. (1994). A lens-mapping framework for understanding the encoding and decoding of interpersonal dispositions in nonverbal behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 398–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Grossard, C., Chaby, L., Hun, S., Pellerin, H., Bourgeois, J., Dapogny, A., Ding, H., Serret, S., Foulon, P., Chetouani, M., Chen, L., Bailly, K., Grynszpan, O., & Cohen, D. (2018). Children facial expression production: Influence of age, gender, emotion subtype, elicitation condition and culture. Frontiers of Psychology, 9, 446. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00446 eCollection 2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Guo, K. (2012). Holistic gaze strategy to categorize facial expression of varying intensities. PLoS One, 7(8), e42585. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042585.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hamel, O., Corre, P., Ploteau, S., Armstrong, O., Rogez, J. M., Robert, R., & Hamel, A. (2012). Ciliary ganglion afferents and efferents variations: A possible explanation of postganglionic mydriasis. Surgical Radiological Anatomy, 34(10), 897–902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hammond, K. R. (1980). Introduction to Brunswikian theory and methods. New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science, 3, 1–11.Google Scholar
- Helminen, T. M., Kaasinen, S. M., & Hietanen, J. K. (2011). Eye contact and arousal: The effects of stimulus duration. Biological Psychology, 88(1), 124–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2010). Infants' neural processing of positive emotion and eye gaze. Social Neuroscience, 5(1), 30–39.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hoehl, S., Palumbo, L., Heinisch, C., & Striano, T. (2008). Infants' attention is biased by emotional expressions and eye gaze direction. Neuroreport, 19(5), 579–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ijuin, K., Umata, I., Kato, T., & Yamamoto, S. (2018). Difference in eye gaze for floor appointment in native- and second-language conversations. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42, 113–128. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0262-3.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ioannou, C., Zein, M. E., Wyart, V., Scheid, I., Amsellem, F., Delorme, R., Chevallier, C., & Grèzes, J. (2017). Shared mechanism for emotion processing in adolescents with and without autism. Scientific Reports, 7, 42696. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep42696.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Isaacowitz, D. M., & Choi, Y. (2011). The malleability of age-related positive gaze preferences: Training to change gaze and mood. Emotion, 11(1), 90–100.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., & Neupert, S. D. (2009). Use of gaze for real-time mood regulation: Effects of age and attentional functioning. Psychology of Aging, 24(4), 989–994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kappas, A., McHugo, G. J., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1989). Componential control and the modulation of emotional experience. Psychophysiology, 26, S37.Google Scholar
- Kappas, A., Bherer, F., & Thériault, M. (2000). Inhibiting facial expressions: Limitations to the voluntary control of facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 24(4), 259–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 394–421.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kret, M. E. (2015). Emotional expressions beyond facial muscle actions. A call for studying autonomic signals and their impact on social perception. Frontiers of Psychology, 6, 711. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00711 eCollection 2015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kret, M. E., & Sjak-Shie, E. E. (2018). Preprocessing pupil size data: Guidelines and code. Behavioral Research Methods. 2018 Jul 10. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1075-y.
- Lance, B. J., & Marsella, S. C. (2010). The expressive gaze model: Using gaze to express emotion. IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, 30(4), 62–73.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McDougal, D. H., & Gamlin, P. D. (2015). Autonomic control of the eye. Comprehensive Physiology, 5(1), 439–473.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Milders, M., Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., & Braun, M. (2011). Detection of emotional faces is modulated by the direction of eye gaze. Emotion, 11(6), 1456–1461.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Myllyneva, A., Ranta, K., & Hietanen, J. K. (2015). Psychophysiological responses to eye contact in adolescents with social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 109, 151–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2014). Dynamic facial expressions allow differentiation of displays intended to convey positive and hubristic pride. Emotion, 14(5), 857–864.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Noh, S. R., Lohani, M., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2011). Deliberate real-time mood regulation in adulthood: The importance of age, fixation and attentional functioning. Cognition and Emotion, 25(6), 998–1013. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.541668.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Novello, B., Renner, A., Maurer, G., Musse, S., & Arteche, A. (2018). Development of the youth emotion picture set. Perception, 47(10–11), 1029–1042. https://doi.org/10.1177/0301006618797226.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pfaff, M. J., Chang, C. C., Patel, A., & Steinbacher, D. M. (2013). Bridge of bone canthopexy: Technique and morphologic assessment. Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive & Aesthetic Surgery, 66(5), 675–681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjps.2012.12.028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pochedly, J. T., Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2012). What emotion does the "facial expression of disgust" express? Emotion, 12(6), 1315–1319.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Raschke, G. F., Bader, R. D., Rieger, U. M., & Schultze-Mosgau, S. (2011). Photo-assisted analysis of blepharoplasty results. Annals of Plastic Surgery, 66(4), 328–333. https://doi.org/10.1097/SAP.0b013e3181fadd71.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Raschke, G. F., Rieger, U. M., Bader, R. D., Schäfer, O., & Schultze-Mosgau, S. (2012). Standardized anthropometric evaluation of ectropion repair results. Journal of Craniofacial Surgery, 23(4), 1032–1037. https://doi.org/10.1097/SCS.0b013e3182505507.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Recio, G., Shmuilovich, O., & Sommer, W. (2014). Should I smile or should I frown? An ERP study on the voluntary control of emotion-related facial expressions. Psychophysiology, 51(8), 789–799. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Reynolds, D. A. J., & Gifford, R. (2001). The sounds and the sights of intelligence: A lens model channel analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 187–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rodger, H., Lao, J., & Caldara, R. (2018). Quantifying facial expression signal and intensity use during development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 174, 41–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.05.005.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Russell, J. A. (1998). Reading emotions from and into faces: Resurrecting a dimensional-contextual perspective. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (p. 298). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Sauer, A., Mothes-Lasch, M., Miltner, W. H., & Straube, T. (2014). Effects of gaze direction, head orientation and valence of facial expression on amygdala activity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(8), 1246–1252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Smith, C. A., & Scott, S. H. (1998). Componential approach to the meaning of facial expressions. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (p. 237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Steinhauer, S. R., Condray, R., & Pless, M. L. (2015). Pharmacological isolation of cognitive components influencing the pupillary light reflex. Journal of Ophthalmology, 2015, 179542. https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/179542.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Teo, L., Lee, S. Y., & Kim, C. Y. (2017). Effect of upgaze on lower eyelid position in Korean patients with congenital ptosis. Journal of Plastic, Reconstructive & Aesthetic Surgery, 70(3), 380–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjps.2016.10.020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Vick, S.-J., Waller, B. M., Parr, L. A., Smith Pasqualini, M. C., & Bard, K. A. (2007). A cross-species comparison of facial morphology and movement in humans and chimpanzees using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-006-0017-z.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Looking happy: The experimental manipulation of a positive visual attention bias. Emotion, 8(1), 121–126.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, L. J., Gillespie, S. M., & Rotshtein, P. (2016). Identification of emotional facial expressions: Effects of expression, intensity, and sex on eye gaze. PLoS One, 11(12), e0168307. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0168307 eCollection 2016.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Children's recognition of disgust in others. Psychology Bulletin, 139(2), 271–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Widmann, A., Schröger, E., & Wetzel, N. (2018). Emotion lies in the eye of the listener: Emotional arousal to novel sounds is reflected in the sympathetic contribution to the pupil dilation response and the P3. Biological Psychology, 133, 10–17.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Xiao, R., Li, X., Li, L., & Wang, Y. (2016). Can we distinguish emotions from faces? Investigation of implicit and explicit processes of peak facial expressions. Frontiers of Psychology, 7, 1330. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01330 eCollection 2016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zloteanu, M., Krumhuber, E. G., & Richardson, D. C. (2018). Detecting genuine and deliberate displays of surprise in static and dynamic faces. Frontiers of Psychology, 9, 1184. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar