Advertisement

Neural correlates of proposers’ fairness perception in punishment and non-punishment economic games

  • Mingliang ChenEmail author
  • Xiaolei Zhu
  • Jing Zhang
  • Ganping Ma
  • Yuanhong Wu
Article
  • 23 Downloads

Abstract

In economic games, people acting as responders always hope to be treated fairly; otherwise they may punish proposers even sacrificing own monetary gains. However, people often treat others unequally when they become proposers, especially in the absence of punishment mechanisms. Will people lose fairness perception when they become proposers? Previous researches mainly focused on the fairness perception of responders, few paid attentions to that of proposers. In present study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) to record the neural activities of participants acting as proposers in the ultimatum game and the dictator game, corresponding to punishment condition and non-punishment condition respectively. In each trial, a participant could choose an offer from three categories: absolutely unfair, relatively unfair and fair. Behavioral results indicate that the proposers would choose more highly unfair offers that are beneficial to themselves in a non-punishment condition than in a punishment condition. However, no matter whether there was punishment mechanism, more highly unfair offers elicited larger MFN. This finding may reflect the existence of the proposers’ unfairness aversion and guilty feeling triggered by intentional violations of social fairness norm and shows that the proposers do not give up the fairness norm in the cockles of the heart even in a non-punishment environment.

Keywords

Proposers’ fairness perception Punishment and non-punishment conditions Ultimatum game and dictator game Event-related potentials Unfairness aversion Guilty feeling 

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant No.70671092, 70971116 and 90924304 from the National Natural Science Foundation of China, and No. 13YJA630006 from the MOE (Ministry of Education) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences of China.

Author Contributions

MC: Main research conceptual framework and reporting.

XZ: Literature collection/review.

JZ: Literature review reorganization and discussion deepening.

GM: Methods and initial reporting.

YW: Design, data collection and analysis.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Andreoni, J. (1990). Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving. The Economic Journal, 100(401), 464–477.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2234133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Andreoni, J., & Miller, J. (2003). Giving according to GARP: An experimental study of rationality and altruism. Econometrica, 70(2), 737–753.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0262.00302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 243–267.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.115.2.243.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bechler, C., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2015). Proportion offered in the dictator and ultimatum games decreases with amount and social distance. Behavioural Processes, 115, 149–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boksem, M. A. S., & De Cremer, D. (2010). Fairness concerns predict medial frontal negativity amplitude in ultimatum bargaining. Social Neuroscience, 5(1), 118–128.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910903202666.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bolton, G. E., & Ockenfels, A. (2000). ERC: A theory of equity, reciprocity, and competition. The American Economic Review, 90(1), 166–193.  https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.90.1.166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bolton, G. E., & Zwick, R. (1995). Anonymity versus punishment in ultimatum bargaining. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 95–121.  https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brenner, T., & Vriend, N. J. (2003). On the behavior of proposers in ultimatum games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 61(4), 617–631.  https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.456980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Camerer, C. F. (2003). Strategizing in the brain. Science, 300(5626), 1673–1675.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1086215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Camerer, C., & Thaler, R. H. (1995). Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators and manners. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(2), 209–219.  https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.9.2.209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2000). What's in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 68(1), 29–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Civai, C., Miniussi, C., & Rumiati, R. I. (2015). Medial prefrontal cortex reacts to unfairness if this damages the self: A tDCS study. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 10(8), 1054–1060.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johnson, T., Mcelreath, R., & Smirnov, O. (2007). Egalitarian motives in humans. Nature, 446(7137), 794–796.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05651.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. De Waal, F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 279–300.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Declerck, C. H., Kiyonari, T., & Boone, C. (2009). Why do responders reject unequal offers in the ultimatum game? An experimental study on the role of perceiving interdependence. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(3), 335–343.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2009.03.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Emmons, R. A., & Colby, P. M. (1995). Emotional conflict and well-being: Relation to perceived availability, daily utilization, and observer reports of social support. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 68(5), 947–957.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.68.5.947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Engel, C. (2011). Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14(4), 583–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Etkin, A., Egner, T., Peraza, D. M., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2006). Resolving emotional conflict: A role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron, 51(6), 871–882.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Falk, A., & Fischbacher, U. (2006). A theory of reciprocity. Games and Economic Behavior, 54(2), 293–315.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2005.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental effects of sanctions on human altruism. Nature, 422(6928), 137–140.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01474.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817–868.  https://doi.org/10.1162/003355399556151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fehr, E., and Schmidt, K. (2005). The economics of fairness, reciprocity and altruism: Experimental evidence and new theories. Handbook of the Economics Giving, Altruism and Reciprocity, Vol 1. (pp. 615-691). Amsterdam, North-Holland.Google Scholar
  23. Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Gächter, S. (2002). Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature, 13(1), 1–25.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1012-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Feng, C., Luo, Y., & Krueger, F. (2015). Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36(2), 591–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fiori, M., Lintas, A., Mesrobian, S., & Villa, A. E. P. (2013). Effect of emotion and personality on deviation from purely rational decision-making. Studies in Computational Intelligence, 474, 129–161.Google Scholar
  26. Friedman, D., Nessler, D., Johnson, R., Jr., Ritter, W., & Bersick, M. (2007). Age-related changes in executive function: An event-related potential (erp) investigation of task-switching. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 15(1), 95–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gabay, A. S., Radua, J., Kempton, M. J., & Mehta, M. A. (2014). The ultimatum game and the brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 549–558.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295(5563), 2279–2282.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1066893.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gilligan, J. (1976). Beyond morality: Psychoanalytic reflections on shame, guilt and love. In T. Lickona (Ed.), Moral development and behavior: Theory, research and social issues (pp. 144–158). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  30. Güroğlu, B., Bos, W. V. D., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Unfair? It depends: Neural correlates of fairness in social context. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(4), 414–423.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq013.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Güroğlu, B., Bos, W. V. D., Dijk, E. V., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., & Crone, E. A. (2011). Dissociable brain networks involved in development of fairness considerations: Understanding intentionality behind unfairness. Neuroimage, 57(2), 634–641.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3(4), 367–388.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(82)90011-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Haselhuhn, M. P., & Mellers, B. A. (2005). Emotions and cooperation in economic games. Cognitive Brain Research, 23(1), 24–33.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & Mcelreath, R. (2001). In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. The American Economic Review, 91(2), 73–78.  https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.91.2.73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Henrich, J., Mcelreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., et al. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312(5781), 1767–1770.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127333.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hewig, J., Kretschmer, N., Trippe, R. H., Hecht, H., Coles, M. G., Holroyd, C. B., & Miltner, W. H. (2011). Why humans deviate from rational choice. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 507–514.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01081.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Mars, R. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2004). Anterior cingulate cortex, selection for action, and error processing. Posner M.i.cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, 219–231.Google Scholar
  38. Hu, J. (2008). The letter, validity and findings of the Chinese version of the questionnaire questionnaire. Chinese Journal of Health Psychology, 16(2), 236–238.Google Scholar
  39. Hu, J., Blue, P. R., Yu, H., Gong, X., Yang, X., Jiang, C., & Zhou, X. (2016). Social status modulates the neural response to unfairness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 1–10.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv086.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Huffmeijer, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2014). Reliability of event-related potentials: The influence of number of trials and electrodes. Physiology & Behavior, 130(6), 13–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Inaba, M., Inoue, Y., Akutsu, S., Takahashi, N., & Yamagishi, T. (2018). Preference and strategy in proposer's prosocial giving in the ultimatum game. PLoS One, 13(3), e0193877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. The Journal of Business, 59(4), 285–300.  https://doi.org/10.1086/296367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Knoch, D., Pascualleone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314(5800), 829–832.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1129156.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(4), 951–956.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4606-06.2007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: The nature and validity of moral stages. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  46. Lee, D. (2008). Game theory and neural basis of social decision making. Nature Neuroscience, 11(4), 404–409.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn2065.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(21), 5796–5804.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4246-06.2007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. McGraw, K. M. (1987). Guilt following transgression: An attribution of responsibility approach. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53(2), 247–256.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.53.2.247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mesrobian, S. K., Bader, M., Götte, L., Villa, A. E. P., & Lintas, A. (2015). Imperfect decision making and risk taking are affected by personality. Decision Making: Uncertainty, Imperfection, Deliberation and Scalability. Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mirowsky, J. (1985). Depression and marital power: An equity model. American Journal of Sociology, 91(3), 557–592.  https://doi.org/10.1086/228314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Mosher, D. L. (1965). Interaction of fear and guilt in inhibiting unacceptable behavior. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 29(2), 161–167.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0021748.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Murnighan, J. K., & Saxon, M. S. (1998). Ultimatum bargaining by children and adults. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19(4), 415–445.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-4870(98)00017-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Nowak, M. A., Page, K. M., & Sigmund, K. (2000). Fairness versus reason in the ultimatum game. Science, 289(5485), 1773–1775.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.289.5485.1773.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Polezzi, D., Daum, I., Rubaltelli, E., Lotto, L., Civai, C., Sartori, G., & Rumiati, R. (2008). Mentalizing in economic decision-making. Behavioural Brain Research, 190(2), 218–223.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Radke, S., & De Bruij, E. (2012). The other side of the coin: Oxytocin decreases the adherence to fairness norms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(4), 193.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00193.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  56. Reuben, E., & Van Winden, F. (2010). Fairness perceptions and prosocial emotions in the power to take. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(6), 908–922.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2010.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rodrigues, J., Ulrich, N., & Hewig, J. (2015). A neural signature of fairness in altruism: A game of theta? Social Neuroscience, 10(2), 192–205.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.977401.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–1758.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Schmitt, P. M. (2004). On perceptions of fairness: The role of valuations, outside options, and information in ultimatum bargaining games. Experimental Economics, 7(1), 49–73.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026210021955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sutton, R. S., & Barton, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An Introduction. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  61. Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Van Winden, F. (2010). Emotional hazard exemplified by taxation-induced anger. Kyklos, 54(2&3), 491–506.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6435.00166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wang, G., Li, J., Li, Z., Wei, M., & Li, S. (2016). Medial frontal negativity reflects advantageous inequality aversion of proposers in the ultimatum game: An erp study. Brain Research, 1639, 38–46.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.02.040.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Weiland, S., Hewig, J., Hecht, H., Mussel, P., & Miltner, W. H. (2012). Neural correlates of fair behavior in interpersonal bargaining. Social Neuroscience, 7(5), 537–551.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.674056.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Wu, Y., Leliveld, M. C., & Zhou, X. (2011). Social distance modulates recipient's fairness consideration in the dictator game: An ERP study. Biological Psychology, 88(2), 253–262.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20), 7398–7401.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0502399102.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  67. Zahn-Waxier, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The origins of guilt. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), The Nebraska symposium on motivation 1988: Socioemotional development (Vol. 36, pp. 182–258). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  68. Zaki, J., & Mitchell, J. P. (2011). Equitable decision making is associated with neural markers of intrinsic value. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(49), 19761–19766.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1112324108.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Zheng, Y., Cheng, X., Xu, J., Zheng, L., Li, L., Yang, G., et al. (2017). Proposers’ economic status affects behavioral and neural responses to unfairness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Zinchenko, O., & Arsalidou, M. (2018). Brain responses to social norms: Meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Human Brain Mapping, 39(2), 955–970.  https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23895.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of ManagementZhejiang UniversityHangzhouChina
  2. 2.Neuromanagement LabZhejiang UniversityHangzhouPeople’s Republic of China

Personalised recommendations