Advertisement

The direct and indirect relationships among kindergarten children’s social mastery motivation, receptive vocabulary, and socioemotional skills

  • Wing-kai FungEmail author
  • Kevin Kien-hoa Chung
Article

Abstract

Social mastery motivation is a critical correlate of children’s vocabulary and socioemotional skills, but little research has investigated the relationships among these factors. The present study investigated the direct relationships between socioemotional skills and the instrumental and expressive indicators of social mastery motivation and the indirect relationships between these variables through receptive vocabulary. The participants were 171 Hong Kong Chinese kindergarten children (76 girls; mean age 3.84 years) and their parents. The children’s social mastery motivation, receptive vocabulary, and non-verbal intelligence were individually assessed in their homes. Both of each child’s parents reported their educational level and child’s socioemotional skills. Path analysis revealed that expressive social mastery motivation had a positive relationship with socioemotional skills and that instrumental social mastery motivation was indirectly related to socioemotional skills through receptive vocabulary. The findings highlight the different relationships between two indicators of social mastery motivation and socioemotional skills, and social mastery motivation may improve children’s vocabulary and socioemotional skills through day-to-day social interactions.

Keywords

Social mastery motivation Receptive vocabulary Socioemotional skills Kindergarten children 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Approval

“All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.”

Informed Consent

“Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.”

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Beck, L., Kumschick, I. R., Eid, M., & Klann-Delius, G. (2012). Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion, 12(3), 503–514.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Cheung, P. P., Lee, K. S., & Lee, L. T. (1997). The development of the 'Cantonese receptive vocabulary Test' for children aged 2–6 in Hong Kong. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 32(1), 127–138.  https://doi.org/10.3109/13682829709021465.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Combs, T. T., & Wachs, T. D. (1993). The construct validity of measures of social mastery motivation. In D. J. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 168–185). New York, NY, US: Routledge.Google Scholar
  4. Denham, S. (2006). Social-emotional competence as support for school readiness: What is it and how do we assess it? Early Education & Development, 17(1), 57–89.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1701_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 258–264.  https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fung, W. K., & Chung, K. K. H. (2019). The roles of social mastery motivation and parental response in preschoolers’ vocabulary knowledge and self-regulation. Early Child Development and Care.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2019.1596902.
  7. Fung, W. K., Chung, K. K. H., & Cheng, R. W. Y. (2018). Gender differences in social mastery motivation and its relationships to vocabulary knowledge, behavioral self-regulation, and socioemotional skills. Early Education and Development, 30(2), 280–293.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1544004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gilmore, L., Cuskelly, M., & Purdie, N. (2003). Mastery motivation: Stability and predictive validity from ages two to eight. Early Education and Development, 14(4), 411–424.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15566935eed1404_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Heep Hong Society (2006). Heep Hong Society Development Assessment Chart 2006 Edition. Hong Kong.Google Scholar
  10. Hirvonen, R., Poikkeus, A., Pakarinen, E., Lerkkanen, M., & Nurmi, J. (2015). Identifying Finnish Children's impulsivity trajectories from kindergarten to grade 4: Associations with academic and socioemotional development. Early Education and Development, 26(5–6), 615–644.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1012190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ho, C. S., Leung, M., & Cheung, H. (2011). Early difficulties of Chinese preschoolers at familial risk for dyslexia: Deficits in oral language, phonological processing skills, and print-related skills. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 17(2), 143–164.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Jeon, H., Wall, S., Peterson, C., Luze, G., Swanson, M., & Graham, S. (2018). Using early indicators of academic risk to predict academic skills and socioemotional functioning at age 10. Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(4), 483–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Józsa, K., & Barrett, K. C. (2018). Affective and social mastery motivation in preschool as predictors of early school success: A longitudinal study. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 45, 81–92.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.05.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Kochanska, G. (1997). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: Implications for early socialization. Child Development, 68(1), 94–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. MacTurk, R. H. (1993). Social and motivational development in deaf and hearing infants. In D. J. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 149–167). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  17. MacTurk, R. H., Hunter, F. T., McCarthy, M. E., Vietze, P. M., & McQuiston, S. (1985). Social mastery motivation in down syndrome and nondelayed infants. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 4(4), 93–109.  https://doi.org/10.1177/027112148500400409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McKown, C., Gumbiner, L. M., Russo, N. M., & Lipton, M. (2009). Social-emotional learning skill, self-regulation, and social competence in typically developing and clinic-referred children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38(6), 858–871.  https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410903258934.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Morgan, G. A., Maslin-Cole, C., Harmon, R. J., Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., Jennings, K. D., Hauser-Cram, P., & Brockman, L. (1993). Parent and teacher perceptions of young children’s mastery motivation : Assessment and review of research. In D. J. Messer (Ed.), Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement and social processes (pp. 109–131). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  20. Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning, and memory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.  https://doi.org/10.4159/9780674041400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Pipp-Siegel, S., Sedey, A. L., VanLeeuwen, A. M., & Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2003). Mastery motivation and expressive language in young children with hearing loss. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8(2), 133–145.  https://doi.org/10.1093/deafed/eng008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. R Core Team (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2018. https://www.R-project.org/ Assessed 13 September 2018.
  23. Raven, J. C. (1998). RPM Manual for Coloured Progressive Matrices (Sec. 2) 1998. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  24. Rispoli, K. M., Mcgoey, K. E., Koziol, N. A., & Schreiber, J. B. (2013). The relation of parenting, child temperament, and attachment security in early childhood to social competence at school entry. Journal of School Psychology, 51(5), 643–658.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2013.05.007.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Salmon, K., Evans, I. M., Moskowitz, S., Grouden, M., Parkes, F., & Miller, E. (2013). The components of young children's emotion knowledge: Which are enhanced by adult emotion talk? Social Development, 22(1), 94–110.  https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Salmon, K., O'Kearney, R., Reese, E., & Fortune, C. (2016). The role of language skill in child psychopathology: Implications for intervention in the early years. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 19(4), 352–367.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-016-0214-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Schmitt, S. A., Pratt, M. E., & McClelland, M. M. (2014). Examining the validity of behavioral self-regulation tools in predicting preschoolers' academic achievement. Early Education and Development, 25(5), 641–660.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2014.850397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Wachs, T. D., & Combs, T. T. (1995). The domains of infant mastery motivation. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations, and applications (pp. 147–164). Westport: Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
  29. Wang, P., Hwang, A., Liao, H., Chen, P., & Hsieh, W. (2011). The stability of mastery motivation and its relationship with home environment in infants and toddlers. Infant Behavior & Development, 34(3), 434–442.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Early Childhood EducationThe Education University of Hong KongTai PoHong Kong

Personalised recommendations