Advertisement

The relationship of aerobic and motor fitness with executive functions in preadolescents

  • Vladimir J. MiloševićEmail author
  • Ana Orlić
  • Danka Purić
  • Snežana Radisavljević Janić
  • Dušanka Lazarević
  • Ivana Milanović
Article
  • 27 Downloads

Abstract

Recent research findings have indicated that aerobic fitness and motor fitness are related to specific executive functions (EF). However, it is not clear whether the relationships of these fitness factors with executive functions are dependent on each other. The goal of this study was thus to investigate the relationship of aerobic fitness, motor fitness, and their interaction with EF in preadolescents. The sample consisted of 137 students (67 boys) aged 9–10 years. Physical fitness assessment included measures of aerobic fitness (20 m shuttle run test) and motor fitness (Hurdle and crawl test), while EF assessment encompassed measures of inhibition (Modified Stroop task), shifting (Smiley task) and updating (Letter memory task). The results showed that motor fitness was positively related to inhibition, r = .22, p < .05, while aerobic fitness was positively related to shifting r = .22, p < .05. Multiple regression and post hoc analyses indicated that aerobic fitness was positively related to shifting only when motor fitness was also high (β = .44, p < .01). This pattern of results could indicate the potential relevance of high levels of both motor and aerobic fitness for EF.

Keywords

Children Shifting Updating Inhibition Cardiorespiratory fitness Skill-related fitness 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the students for participating in this study, as well as the teachers of Elementary school “Svetozar Miletić” Zemun, Serbia who kindly helped us with gathering the data. Special thanks go to Aleksandar Ružić and Marko Milojević for motor testing and expertise in children’s anthropometrics and biomechanics issues.

This paper is the result of the projects ‘Improving the quality and accessibility of education in modernization processes in Serbia’ No 47008 and ‘The effects of physical activity application to locomotor, metabolic, psychosocial and educational status with the population of the Republic of Serbia’ No III47015 financially supported by the Ministry of Education and Science of the Republic of Serbia.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Belgrade, Faculty of Sport and Physical Education, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Additional informed consent was obtained for all individual participants for whom identifying information is included in this article.

References

  1. Aadland, K. N., Moe, V. F., Aadland, E., Anderssen, S. A., Resaland, G. K., & Ommundsen, Y. (2017). Relationships between physical activity, sedentary time, aerobic fitness, motor skills and executive function and academic performance in children. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 12, 10–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adkins, D. L., Boychuk, J., Remple, M. S., & Kleim, J. A. (2006). Motor training induces experience-specific patterns of plasticity across motor cortex and spinal cord. Journal of Applied Physiology, 101(6), 1776–1782.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions: Sage.Google Scholar
  4. Alesi, M., Bianco, A., Luppina, G., Palma, A., & Pepi, A. (2016). Improving children's coordinative skills and executive functions: The effects of a football exercise program. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 122(1), 27–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bailey, C. E. (2007). Cognitive accuracy and intelligent executive function in the brain and in business. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118(1), 122–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review, 30(4), 331–351.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Borella, E., Carretti, B., & Pelegrina, S. (2010). The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(6), 541–552.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Brydges, C. R., Reid, C. L., Fox, A. M., & Anderson, M. (2012). A unitary executive function predicts intelligence in children. Intelligence, 40(5), 458–469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Buck, S. M., Hillman, C. H., & Castelli, D. M. (2008). The relation of aerobic fitness to stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 40(1), 166–172.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., & Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Reports, 100(2), 126–131.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Castelli, D. M., Hillman, C. H., Hirsch, J., Hirsch, A., & Drollette, E. (2011). FIT kids: Time in target heart zone and cognitive performance. Preventive Medicine, 52, S55–S59.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Kim, J. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., et al. (2010a). A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Research, 1358, 172–183.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., VanPatter, M., Voss, M. W., Pontifex, M. B., et al. (2010b). Basal ganglia volume is associated with aerobic fitness in preadolescent children. Developmental Neuroscience, 32(3), 249–256.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Chaddock, L., Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., & Kramer, A. F. (2011). A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 975–985.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Chaddock-Heyman, L., Erickson, K. I., Holtrop, J. L., Voss, M. W., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2014). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater white matter integrity in children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(584), 1–7.Google Scholar
  18. Chang, Y.-K., Tsai, Y.-J., Chen, T.-T., & Hung, T.-M. (2013). The impacts of coordinative exercise on executive function in kindergarten children: An ERP study. Experimental Brain Research, 225(2), 187–196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Crova, C., Struzzolino, I., Marchetti, R., Masci, I., Vannozzi, G., Forte, R., et al. (2014). Cognitively challenging physical activity benefits executive function in overweight children. Journal of Sports Sciences, 32(3), 201–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. de Bruijn, A., Hartman, E., Kostons, D., Visscher, C., & Bosker, R. (2018). Exploring the relations among physical fitness, executive functioning, and low academic achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 204–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71(1), 44–56.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 335–341.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Donnelly, J. E., Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D., Etnier, J. L., Lee, S., Tomporowski, P., et al. (2016). Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: A systematic review. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 48(6), 1197.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Erickson, K. I., Leckie, R. L., & Weinstein, A. M. (2014). Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiology of Aging, 35, S20–S28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fedewa, A. L., & Ahn, S. (2011). The effects of physical activity and physical fitness on children's achievement and cognitive outcomes: A meta-analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82(3), 521–535.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., et al. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive functioning in late adolescence. Psychological Science, 18(10), 893–900.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Knight, C., & Stegmann, Z. (2004). Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(1), 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hartman, E., Smith, J., Houwen, S., & Visscher, C. (2017). Skill-related physical fitness versus aerobic fitness as a predictor of executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities or borderline intellectual functioning. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 64, 1–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., & Castelli, D. M. (2009a). Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: Event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 114–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E., & Kramer, A. F. (2009b). The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 159(3), 1044–1054.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Castelli, D. M., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., et al. (2014). Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics, 134(4), e1063–e1071.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Janssen, I., & LeBlanc, A. G. (2010). Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 7(1), 40.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Khan, N. A., & Hillman, C. H. (2014). The relation of childhood physical activity and aerobic fitness to brain function and cognition: A review. Pediatric Exercise Science, 26(2), 138–146.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kubesch, S., Walk, L., Spitzer, M., Kammer, T., Lainburg, A., Heim, R., et al. (2009). A 30-minute physical education program improves students’ executive attention. Mind, Brain, and Education, 3(4), 235–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Lambourne, K., & Tomporowski, P. (2010). The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Research, 1341, 12–24.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Leger, L. A., & Lambert, J. (1982). A maximal multistage 20-m shuttle run test to predict\ dot VO2 max. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 49(1), 1–12.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Leger, L. A., Mercier, D., Gadoury, C., & Lambert, J. (1988). The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness. Journal of Sports Sciences, 6(2), 93–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lui, M., & Tannock, R. (2007). Working memory and inattentive behaviour in a community sample of children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Marchetti, R., Forte, R., Borzacchini, M., Vazou, S., Tomporowski, P. D., & Pesce, C. (2015). Physical and motor fitness, sport skills and executive function in adolescents: A moderated prediction model. Psychology, 6(14), 1915–1929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1), 167–202.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Milosevic, V., & Petrovic, A. (2015). Reliability of field-based tests for monitoring and assessing physical fitness in preschool children. Serbian Journal of Sports Sciences, 9(1), 20–26.Google Scholar
  45. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Moreau, D. (2012). The role of motor processes in three-dimensional mental rotation: Shaping cognitive processing via sensorimotor experience. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(3), 354–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Moreau, D. (2013). Motor expertise modulates movement processing in working memory. Acta Psychologica, 142(3), 356–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Niederer, I., Kriemler, S., Gut, J., Hartmann, T., Schindler, C., Barral, J., et al. (2011). Relationship of aerobic fitness and motor skills with memory and attention in preschoolers (Ballabeina): A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. BMC Pediatrics, 11(1), 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Oberer, N., Gashaj, V., & Roebers, C. M. (2018). Executive functions, visual-motor coordination, physical fitness and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations in typically developing children. Human Movement Science, 58, 69–79.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ortega, F. B., Cadenas-Sánchez, C., Sánchez-Delgado, G., Mora-González, J., Martínez-Téllez, B., Artero, E. G., et al. (2015). Systematic review and proposal of a field-based physical fitness-test battery in preschool children: The PREFIT battery. Sports Medicine, 45(4), 533–555.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pallier, G., Wilkinson, R., Danthiir, V., Kleitman, S., Knezevic, G., Stankov, L., et al. (2002). The role of individual differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. The Journal of General Psychology, 129(3), 257–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Pesce, C. (2012). Shifting the focus from quantitative to qualitative exercise characteristics in exercise and cognition research. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 34(6), 766–786.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Pesce, C., Crova, C., Marchetti, R., Struzzolino, I., Masci, I., Vannozzi, G., et al. (2013). Searching for cognitively optimal challenge point in physical activity for children with typical and atypical motor development. Mental Health and Physical Activity, 6(3), 172–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1332–1345.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Riggs, N. R., Spruijt-Metz, D., Sakuma, K.-L., Chou, C.-P., & Pentz, M. A. (2010). Executive cognitive function and food intake in children. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 42(6), 398–403.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Kane, R., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012). An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 54(11), 1025–1031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Roebers, C. M., Röthlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Michel, E., & Jäger, K. (2014). The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children’s transition to school: A latent variable approach. Human Movement Science, 33, 284–297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schmidt, M., Jäger, K., Egger, F., Roebers, C. M., & Conzelmann, A. (2015). Cognitively engaging chronic physical activity, but not aerobic exercise, affects executive functions in primary school children: A group-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 37(6), 575–591.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Schmidt, M., Egger, F., Benzing, V., Jäger, K., Conzelmann, A., Roebers, C. M., et al. (2017). Disentangling the relationship between children’s motor ability, executive function and academic achievement. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182845.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Schuch, S., Bayliss, A. P., Klein, C., & Tipper, S. P. (2010). Attention modulates motor system activation during action observation: Evidence for inhibitory rebound. Experimental Brain Research, 205(2), 235–249.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sibley, B. A., & Etnier, J. L. (2003). The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science, 15(3), 243–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at Adolescence 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
  65. Tomporowski, P. D., McCullick, B., Pendleton, D. M., & Pesce, C. (2015). Exercise and children's cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 4(1), 47–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Utesch, T., Dreiskämper, D., Strauss, B., & Naul, R. (2017). The development of the physical fitness construct across childhood. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 28(1), 212–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. van der Niet, A. G., Smith, J., Oosterlaan, J., Scherder, E. J., Hartman, E., & Visscher, C. (2016). Effects of a cognitively demanding aerobic intervention during recess on children’s physical fitness and executive functioning. Pediatric Exercise Science, 28(1), 64–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Verburgh, L., Königs, M., Scherder, E. J., & Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 48, 973–979.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Kim, J. S., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., et al. (2011). Aerobic fitness is associated with greater efficiency of the network underlying cognitive control in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 199, 166–176.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Sport and Physical EducationUniversity of BelgradeBelgradeSerbia
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Faculty of PhilosophyUniversity of BelgradeBelgradeSerbia

Personalised recommendations