Advertisement

Current Psychology

, Volume 38, Issue 5, pp 1304–1309 | Cite as

Effects of SOA and Age on the Inhibition of Return in a Localization Task

  • A. I. AgustiEmail author
  • E. Satorres
  • A. Pitarque
  • J. C. Meléndez
Article

Abstract

The objective of this research was to study attentional tendencies, specifically the effect of inhibition of return (IOR), comparing a sample of young adults (N = 72) and elderly (N = 74). A task of attention discrimination was used in combination with SOAs (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony) of 300, 750,100 and 1500 ms are used. The results showed that both groups obtained higher reaction times (RT) for the cued assays than the uncued, these data indicated the existence of an IOR effect. In addition, the young adults continued a course of constant decrease in their RT as the SOA increases, however, the older ones also followed a decrease in their RT until reaching the SOA of 1500 ms in which they experienced a significant increase. The observed difference between groups in the 1500 ms SOA indicated that older adults have an IOR effect on long SOAs greater and more durable IOR effect than younger.

Keywords

IOR Aging Young Older RT 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.

References

  1. Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bao, Y., Zhou, J., & Fu, L. (2004). Aging and the time course of inhibition of return in a static environment. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 64, 403–414.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Berger, A., Henik, A., & Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 207–221.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.2.207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Castel, A. D., Chasteen, A. L., Scialfa, C. T., & Pratt, J. (2003). Adult age differences in the time course of inhibition of return. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58, 256–259.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.5.P256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chica, A. B., & Lupiáñez, J. (2004). Inhibición de retorno sin retorno de la atención. Psicothema, 16, 248–254.Google Scholar
  6. Chica, A. B., Lupiáñez, J., & Bartolomeo, P. (2006). Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 1015–1034.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290600588277.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini Mental State. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinical. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Gabay, S., & Henik, A. (2010). Temporal expectancy modulates inhibition of return in a discrimination task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 47–51.  https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.1.47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hartley, A. A., & Kieley, J. M. (1995). Adult age differences in the inhibition of return of visual attention. Psychology and Aging, 10, 670–683.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.670.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 193–225.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227–249). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Kaur, G. P., & Singh, R. (2014). Inhibition of return and ageing: An overview. Indian Journal of Health and Wellbeing, 5, 1087–1090.Google Scholar
  13. Klein, R. M., & Taylor, T. L. (1994). Categories of cognitive Inhibition with reference to attention. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and lenguage (pp. 113–150). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  14. Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 138–147.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01452-2.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Langley, L. K., Fuentes, L. J., Hochhalter, A. K., Brandt, J., & Overmier, J. B. (2001). Inhibition of return in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: Performance as a function of task demands and stimulus timing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 431–446.  https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.4.431.1235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Langley, L. K., Vivas, A. B., Fuentes, L. J., & Bagne, A. G. (2005). Differential age effects on attention-based inhibition: Inhibitory tagging and inhibition of return. Psychology and Aging, 20, 356–360.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.356.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Langley, L. K., Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A. B., & Saville, A. L. (2007). Aging and temporal patterns of inhibition of return. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, 71–77.  https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.2.P71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Langley, L. K., Gayzur, N. D., Saville, A. L., Morlock, S. L., & Bagne, A. G. (2011a). Spatial distribution of attentional inhibition is not altered in healthy aging. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 766–783.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-010-0059-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Langley, L. K., Friesen, C. K., Saville, A. L., & Ciernia, A. T. (2011b). Timing of reflexive visuospatial orienting in young, young-old, and old-old adults. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 1546–1561.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0108-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lupiáñez, J., Milliken, B., Solano, C., Weaver, B., & Tipper, S. P. (2001). On the strategic modulation of the time course of facilitation and inhibition of return. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54, 753–773.  https://doi.org/10.1080/713755990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Poliakoff, E., Coward, R. S., Lowe, C., & O’Boyle, D. J. (2007). The effect of age on inhibition of return is independent of non-ocular response inhibition. Neuropsychologia, 45, 387–396.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Posner, M. I. (2016). Orienting of attention: Then and now. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 1864–1875.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.937446.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. X, pp. 531–556). Hillsade: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. Radloff, L. S. (1997). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401.  https://doi.org/10.1177/014662167700100306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tanaka, Y., & Shimojo, S. (1996). Location vs feature: Reaction time reveals dissociation between two visual functions. Vision Research, 36, 2125–2140.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(95)00272-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Terry, K. M., Valdes, L. A., & Neill, W. T. (1994). Does “inhibition of return” occur in discrimination tasks? Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 279–286.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03207599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Tsuchida, N. (2005). Inhibition of return using location discrimination in two age groups. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, 554–558.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.100.2.554-558.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. 4Verhaeghen, P. (2011). Aging and executive control: Reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 174–180.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411408772.
  29. Wu, C., Dagg, P., Ward, C., & Crawford, M. (2011). Inhibition of return in older adults with schizophrenia: Does age matter? Aging and Society: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 1, 49–60.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • A. I. Agusti
    • 1
    Email author
  • E. Satorres
    • 1
  • A. Pitarque
    • 2
  • J. C. Meléndez
    • 1
  1. 1.Developmental Psychology, Faculty of PsychologyUniversity of ValenciaValenciaSpain
  2. 2.Department of Methodology, Faculty of PsychologyUniversity of ValenciaValenciaSpain

Personalised recommendations