Advertisement

Current Psychology

, Volume 38, Issue 1, pp 92–99 | Cite as

The Relation of Dietary Restraint and Affect with Food Choice and the Experience of Guilt after Eating

  • Kyle P. De YoungEmail author
  • Mary Zander
  • Terra Towne
  • Nicole M. Della Longa
  • Lindsey Hovrud
  • Erin Murtha-Berg
Article

Abstract

This study examined the relation of affect and the desire to restrict intake (i.e., dietary restraint) with food choice and the experience of guilt after eating. We hypothesized that restraint would predict choosing lower-calorie foods except when individuals with high restraint experienced negative affect, and guilt after eating would be associated with restraint among individuals with high negative affect. Participants (N = 309) completed measures of restraint and eating psychopathology before experiencing a positive, negative, or no mood induction. Participants then chose foods from four menus and ate one food item before rating their momentary guilt. Restraint predicted choosing lower calorie foods, but negative affect did not influence this relationship. An interaction of restraint and negative mood predicted guilt after eating. Thus, dietary restraint predicts restriction at the choice-level, and individuals high on restraint may interpret negative emotion while eating as a failure of behavioral control, producing guilt.

Keywords

Dietary restraint Negative affect Eating psychopathology Guilt 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Funding

There was no funding body for this study.

Research Involving Human Participants

All procedures performed in this study were approved by the local institutional review board.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

In addition to being an assistant professor at the University of Wyoming, Dr. De Young is the Director for Communications for the Academy for Eating Disorders and Adjunct Scientist at the Neuropsychiatric Research Institute. Dr. Zander declares that she has no conflict of interest. Ms. Towne declares that she has no conflict of interest. Ms. Della Longa declares that she has no conflict of interest. Ms. Hovrud declares that she has no conflict of interest. Ms. Murtha-Berg declares that she has no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  3. Berg, K. C., Crosby, R. D., Cao, L., Peterson, C. B., Engel, S. G., Mitchell, J. E., & Wonderlich, S. A. (2013). Facets of negative affect prior to and following binge-only, purge-only, and binge/purge events in women with bulimia nervosa. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 111–118. doi: 10.1037/a0029703.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. De Young, K. P., Lavender, J. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2013). Moderators of post-binge eating negative emotion in eating disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(3), 323–328. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.11.012.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Eich, E., & Metcalfe, J. (1989). Mood dependent memory for internal versus external events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(3), 443–455. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.3.443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Engel, S. G., Wonderlich, S. A., Crosby, R. D., Mitchell, J. E., Crow, S., Peterson, C. B., et al. (2013). The role of affect in the maintenance of anorexia nervosa: Evidence from a naturalistic assessment of momentary behaviors and emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(3), 709–719. doi: 10.1037/a0034010.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (2008). Eating disorder examination questionnaire (EDE-Q 6.0). In C. G. Fairburn (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders (pp. 309–314). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  8. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. –G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146.
  9. Goodrick, G. K., Poston II, W. S. C., Kimball, K. T., Reeves, R. S., & Foreyt, J. P. (1998). Nondieting versus dieting treatment for overweight binge-eating women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(2), 363–368. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.2.363.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Habhab, S., Sheldon, J. P., & Loeb, R. C. (2009). The relationship between stress, dietary restraint, and food preferences in women. Appetite, 52(2), 437–444. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Haedt-Matt, A. A., & Keel, P. K. (2011). Revisiting the affect regulation model of binge eating: A meta-analysis of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Psychological Bulletin, 137(4), 660–681. doi: 10.1037/a0023660.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 86–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (2004). The self-regulation of eating: Theoretical and practical problems. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 492–508). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Laessle, R. G., Tuschl, R. J., Kotthaus, B. C., & Pirke, K. M. (1989). A comparison of the validity of three scales for the assessment of dietary restraint. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98(4), 504–507. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.98.4.504.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Lowe, M. R., & Levine, A. S. (2005). Eating motives and the controversy over dieting: Eating less than needed versus less than wanted. Obesity Research, 13(5), 797–806. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.90.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (2013). Managing overweight and obesity in adults. Retrieved from http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/guidelines.
  17. Oliver, G., & Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiology & Behavior, 66(3), 511–515. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00322-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1985). Dieting and binge eating: A causal analysis. American Psychologist, 40, 193–204. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.2.193.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1993). Etiology of binge eating: Psychological mechanisms. In C. G. Fairburn & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 173–205). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  20. Presnell, K., & Stice, E. (2003). An experimental test of the effect of weight-loss dieting on bulimic pathology: Tipping the scales in a different direction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(1), 166–170. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.1.166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Riener, C. R., Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Clore, G. (2011). An effect of mood on the perception of geographical slant. Cognition and Emotion, 25(1), 174–182. doi: 10.1080/02699931003738026.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Ritz, T., Trueba, A. F., Simon, E., & Auchus, R. J. (2014). Increases in exhaled nitric oxide after acute stress: Associations with measures of negative affect and depressed mood. Psychosomatic Medicine, 76(9), 716–725. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Roemmich, J. N., Wright, S. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2002). Dietary restraint and stress-induced snacking in youth. Obesity Research, 10(11), 1120–1126. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Roemmich, J. N., Lambiase, M. J., Lobarinas, C. L., & Balantekin, K. N. (2011). Interactive effects of dietary restraint and adiposity on stress-induced eating and the food choice of children. Eating Behaviors, 12(4), 309–312. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.07.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Sanftner, J. L., & Crowther, J. H. (1998). Variability in self-esteem, moods, shame, and guilt in women who binge. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23(4), 391–397. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199805)23:4<391::AID-EAT6>3.0.CO;2-D.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2006). The approach and avoidance function of guilt and shame emotions: Comparing reactions to self-caused and other-caused wrongdoing. Motivation and Emotion, 30(1), 43–56. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9006-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Siemer, M. (2005). Mood-congruent cognitions constitute mood experience. Emotion, 5(3), 296–308. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.296.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Smyth, J. M., Wonderlich, S. A., Heron, K. E., Sliwinski, M. J., Crosby, R. D., Mitchell, J. E., & Engel, S. G. (2007). Daily and momentary mood and stress are associated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in the natural environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(4), 629–638. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.629.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Stice, E. (2001). A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: Mediating effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 124–135. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.124.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Stice, E., Nemeroff, C., & Shaw, H. E. (1996). Test of the dual pathway model of bulimia nervosa: Evidence for dietary restraint and affect regulation mechanisms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15(3), 340–363. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1996.15.3.340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Stice, E., Killen, J. D., Hayward, C., & Taylor, C. B. (1998). Age of onset for binge eating and purging during late adolescence: A 4-year survival analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(4), 671–675. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.107.4.671.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in adolescent girls: A 2-year prospective investigation. Health Psychology, 21(2), 131–138. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.131.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Stice, E., Fisher, M., & Lowe, M. R. (2004). Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of acute dietary restriction? Unobtrusive observational data suggest not. Psychological Assessment, 16(1), 51–59. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.51.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Stice, E., Presnell, K., Groesz, L., & Shaw, H. (2005). Effects of a weight maintenance diet on bulimic symptoms in adolescent girls: An experimental test of the dietary restraint theory. Health Psychology, 24(4), 402–412. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.4.402.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Stice, E., Cooper, J. A., Schoeller, D. A., Tappe, K., & Lowe, M. R. (2007). Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of moderate-to long-term dietary restriction? Objective biological and behavioral data suggest not. Psychological Assessment, 19(4), 449–458. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.4.449.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Stice, E., Sysko, R., Roberto, C. A., & Allison, S. (2010). Are dietary restraint scales valid measures of dietary restriction? Additional objective behavioral and biological data suggest not. Appetite, 54(2), 331–339. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.12.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. van Strien, T., Engels, R. C., van Staveren, W., & Herman, C. P. (2006). The validity of dietary restraint scales: Comment on Stice et al. (2004). Psychological Assessment, 18(1), 89–94. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.1.89
  38. Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). The epistemic benefits of trait-consistent mood states: An analysis of extraversion and mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(3), 663–677. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.663.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Tepper, B. J., Trail, A. C., & Shaffer, S. E. (1996). Diet and physical activity in restrained eaters. Appetite, 27(1), 51–64. doi: 10.1006/appe.1996.0033.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Tilghman-Osborne, C., Cole, D. A., & Felton, J. W. (2010). Definition and measurement of guilt: Implications for clinical research and practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(5), 536–546. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Tracy, L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 103–125. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tuorila, H., Kramer, F. M., & Engell, D. (2001). The choice of fat-free vs. regular-fat fudge: The effects of liking for the alternative and the restraint status. Appetite, 37(1), 27–32. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0410.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule–expanded form. Iowa City: The University of Iowa.Google Scholar
  44. Zellner, D. A., Loaiza, S., Gonzalez, Z., Pita, J., Morales, J., Pecora, D., & Wolf, A. (2006). Food selection changes under stress. Physiology & Behavior, 87(4), 789–793. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kyle P. De Young
    • 1
    Email author
  • Mary Zander
    • 2
  • Terra Towne
    • 2
  • Nicole M. Della Longa
    • 2
  • Lindsey Hovrud
    • 3
  • Erin Murtha-Berg
    • 2
  1. 1.University of WyomingLaramieUSA
  2. 2.University of North DakotaGrand ForksUSA
  3. 3.University of South DakotaVermillionUSA

Personalised recommendations