Advertisement

Topoi

pp 1–12 | Cite as

What is Symbolic Cognition?

  • Ronald J. PlanerEmail author
Article

Abstract

Humans’ capacity for so-called symbolic cognition is often invoked by evolutionary theorists, and in particular archaeologists, when attempting to explain human cognitive and behavioral uniqueness. But what is meant by “symbolic cognition” is often left underspecified. In this article, I identify and discuss three different ways in which the notion of symbolic cognition might be construed, each of them quite distinct. Getting clear on the nature of symbolic cognition is a necessary first step in determining what symbolic cognition might plausibly explain.

Keywords

Symbolic cognition The evolution of language Inner speech Language of thought Meaning conventions 

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Kim Sterelny and Lauren Reed for comments on an early draft of this article. I would also like to thank the anonymous referees for their feedback.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The author reports no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

References

  1. Atkin A (2013) Peirce’s theory of signs. In: Zalta EN (ed) The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, summer edition. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2013/entries/peirce-semiotics/
  2. Baars BJ (1997) In the theatre of consciousness. Global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. J Conscious Stud 4(4):292–309Google Scholar
  3. Baars BJ (2005) Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Prog Brain Res 150:45–53Google Scholar
  4. Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behav Brain Sci 22(4):637–660Google Scholar
  5. Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2016) Why only us: language and evolution? MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  6. Bowles S, Gintis H (2013) A cooperative species: human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  7. Boyd R (2017) A different kind of animal: how culture transformed our species. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  8. Carruthers P (2006) The architecture of the mind. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  9. Chase P (2003) Comment on “The origin of modern behavior: a review and critique of models and test implications” by Henshilwood et al. Curr Anthropol 44(5):637Google Scholar
  10. Currie A, Killin A (2019) From things to thinking: cognitive archaeology. Mind Lang 34(2):263–279Google Scholar
  11. Damasio A (2012) Self comes to mind: constructing the conscious brain. Vintage, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  12. Deacon TW (1998) The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. WW Norton & Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  13. Dennett DC (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Company, BostonGoogle Scholar
  14. Everett D (2017) How language began: the story of humanity’s greatest invention. Profile Books, LondonGoogle Scholar
  15. Fields C (2002) Why do we talk to ourselves? J Exp Theor Artif Intell 14(4):255–272Google Scholar
  16. Fodor JA (1975) The language of thought. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Fodor JA (1998) Concepts: where cognitive science went wrong. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Fodor JA, Pylyshyn ZW (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis. Cognition 28(1–2):3–71Google Scholar
  19. Gallistel CR, King AP (2011) Memory and the computational brain: why cognitive science will transform neuroscience, vol 6. Wiley, New York, p 2011Google Scholar
  20. Gärdenfors P (1995) Cued and detached representations in animal cognition. Behav Proc 35(1–3):263–273Google Scholar
  21. Goldstone RL, Barsalou LW (1998) Reuniting perception and conception. Cognition 65(2-3):231–262Google Scholar
  22. Henshilwood CS et al (2003) The origin of modern human behavior: critique of the models and their test implications. Curr Anthropol 44(5):627–651Google Scholar
  23. Hurford JR (2014) Origins of language: a slim guide. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  24. Lewis D (1969) Convention Cambridge. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  25. Malafouris L (2013) How things shape the mind. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  26. McBrearty S, Brooks AS (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol 39(5):453–563Google Scholar
  27. Mercier H, Sperber D (2017) The enigma of reason. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  28. Mithen S (1996) The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion and science. Thames & Hudson, Great BritainGoogle Scholar
  29. Mithen S (2005) The singing Neanderthals: the origins of music, language, mind and body. Weidenfeld & Nicolson, Great BritainGoogle Scholar
  30. Newell A (1980) Physical symbol systems. Cognit Sci 4(2):135–183Google Scholar
  31. Noble W, Davidson I (1991) The evolutionary emergence of modern human behaviour: language and its archaeology. Man 1991:223–253Google Scholar
  32. Nowell A (2010) Defining behavioral modernity in the context of Neandertal and anatomically modern human populations. Annu Rev Anthropol 39:437–452Google Scholar
  33. Peirce CSS (ed) (1883) Studies in logic. By members of the Johns Hopkins University. Little, Brown, and Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  34. Planer RJ (forthcoming) The evolution of languages of thought. Biol PhilosGoogle Scholar
  35. Planer RJ, Kalkman D (2019) Arbitrary signals and cognitive complexity. Br J Philos Sci.  https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axz018
  36. Prinz JJ, Barsolou LW (2002) Acquisition and productivity in perceptual symbol systems: an account of mundane creativity. Creativity, cognition, and knowledge: an interaction. Praeger, Santa Barbara, p 105Google Scholar
  37. Renfrew C (2004) Towards a theory of material engagement. Rethinking materiality: the engagement of mind with the material world. McDonald Institute, Cambridge, pp 23–31Google Scholar
  38. Rossano MJ (2010) Making friends, making tools, and making symbols. Curr Anthropol 51(S1):S89–S98Google Scholar
  39. Selfridge OG, Neisser U (1960) Pattern recognition by machine. Sci Am 203(2):60–69Google Scholar
  40. Shanon B (1998) What is the function of consciousness? J Conscious Stud 5(3):295–308Google Scholar
  41. Skyrms B (2010) Signals: evolution, learning, and information. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  42. Soffer O (2009) Defining modernity, establishing rubicons, imagining the other—and the Neanderthal enigma. Sourcebook of Paleolithic transitions. Springer, New York, pp 43–64Google Scholar
  43. Stiner MC (2017) Love and death in the Stone Age: what constitutes first evidence of mortuary treatment of the human body? Biol Theory 12(4):248–261Google Scholar
  44. Stringer C, Gamble C (1993) In search of the Neanderthals: solving the puzzle of human origins. Thames and Hudson, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  45. Tattersall I (2008) An evolutionary framework for the acquisition of symbolic cognition by Homo sapiens. Compar Cognit Behav Rev 3:99–114Google Scholar
  46. Vicente A, Martinez Manrique F (2011) Inner speech: nature and functions. Philos Compass 6(3):209–219Google Scholar
  47. Vygotsky LS (2012 [1932]) Thought and language. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  48. Wadley L (2001) What is cultural modernity? A general view and a South African perspective from Rose Cottage Cave. Camb Archaeol J 11(2):201–221Google Scholar
  49. Wilkins DP (1997) Alternative representations of space: arrernte narratives in sand. The visual narrative reader. Bloomsbury, London, pp 252–281Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Philosophy and Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of LanguageThe Australian National UniversityCanberraAustralia

Personalised recommendations