Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 32, Issue 5, pp 1243–1256 | Cite as

The effects of emotional significance of foveal words on the parafoveal processing of N + 2 words in reading Chinese sentences

  • Ming YanEmail author
  • Werner Sommer
Article
  • 103 Downloads

Abstract

The emotional significance of stimuli has a strong effect on lexical processing across different reading paradigms. In the present study, we investigated whether foveal and parafoveal lexical processing is influenced by foveal emotional words (positive, negative, or neutral) during the reading of Chinese sentences. We tested word N + 2 preview effect by manipulating the visibility of the upcoming word, located two words away from the foveal word. Processing benefits due to valid parafoveal preview were found for all three valence classes of foveal words. Most interestingly, for negative as compared to both neutral and positive foveal target words, the parafoveal preview effect was reduced when preview duration had been long. These findings suggest that negative words are more likely to attract readers’ attention, narrowing the attentional spotlight to the fovea as affected information becomes activated during word processing. We discuss implications for the notion of attention attraction due to emotional content.

Keywords

Eye movement Emotion Reading Chinese 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Hua Shu and Wei Zhou for their assistance during data collection and Reinhold Kliegl for his comments on the manuscript. The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Funding

This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft under Grant KL 955/18.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors report no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beijing Language Institute Publisher. (1986). Modern Chinese word frequency dictionary (in Chinese). Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Publisher.Google Scholar
  3. Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: Orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Lee, S. C. (1997). Attentional biases for negative information in induced and naturally occurring dysphoria. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 911–927.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Chen, H.-C., & Shu, H. (2001). Lexical activation during the recognition of Chinese characters: Evidence against early phonological activation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 511–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dimigen, O., Kliegl, R., & Sommer, W. (2012). Trans-saccadic parafoveal preview benefits in fluent reading: A study with fixation-related brain potentials. Neuroimage, 62, 381–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dixon, P., & Bortolussi, M. (2011). The scientific study of literature: What can, has, and should be done. Scientific Study of Literature, 1(1), 59–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research, 43, 1035–1045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Frühholz, S., Jellinghaus, A., & Herrmann, M. (2011). Time course of implicit processing and explicit processing of emotional faces and emotional words. Biological Psychology, 87, 265–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hakemulder, J. F. (2004). Foregrounding and its effect on readers’ perception. Discourse Processes, 38(2), 193–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Henderson, J. M., & Ferreira, F. (1990). Effects of foveal processing difficulty on the perceptual span in reading: Implications for attention and eye movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 417–429.Google Scholar
  13. Ho, M. C., Li, S. H., & Yeh, S. L. (2016). Early attentional bias for negative words when competition is induced. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(4), 1030–1042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Remef. [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://github.com/hohenstein/remef.
  15. Hoosain, R. (1991). Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
  16. Hyönä, J., & Häikiö, T. (2005). Is emotional content obtained from parafoveal words during reading? An eye movement analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Inhoff, A. W., & Liu, W. (1998). The perceptual span and oculomotor activity during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 20–34.Google Scholar
  18. Inhoff, A. W., & Rayner, K. (1986). Parafoveal word processing during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word frequency. Perception and Psychophysics, 40, 431–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kissler, J., Assadollahi, R., & Herbert, C. (2006). Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: Insights from ERP studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 147–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kissler, J., Herbert, C., Peyk, P., & Junghöfer, M. (2007). Buzzwords: Early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychological Science, 18(6), 475–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kliegl, R., Hohenstein, S., Yan, M., & McDonald, S. A. (2013). How preview space/time translates into preview cost/benefit for fixation durations during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 581–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kliegl, R., Masson, M. E. J., & Richter, E. M. (2010a). A linear mixed model analysis of masked repetition priming. Visual Cognition, 18, 655–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kliegl, R., Wei, P., Dambacher, M., Yan, M., & Zhou, X. (2010b). Experimental effects and individual differences in linear mixed models: Estimating the relationship between spatial, object, and attraction effects in visual attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 238.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00238.Google Scholar
  24. Kuperman, V., Estes, Z., Brysbaert, M., & Warriner, A. B. (2014). Emotion and language: Valence and arousal affect word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1065–1081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Li, M. F., Gao, X. Y., Chou, T. L., & Wu, J. T. (2017). Neighborhood frequency effect in Chinese word recognition: Evidence from naming and lexical decision. The Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 227–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Li, M. F., Lin, W. C., Chou, T. L., Yang, F. L., & Wu, J. T. (2015). The role of orthographic neighborhood size effects in Chinese word recognition. The Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 219–236.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9340-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Marx, C., Hawelka, S., Schuster, S., & Hutzler, F. (2015). An incremental boundary study on parafoveal preprocessing in children reading aloud: Parafoveal masks overestimate the preview benefit. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27, 549–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2017). Balancing Type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 17, 578–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Palazova, M., Mantwill, K., Sommer, W., & Schacht, A. (2011). Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2766–2775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing [Computer software].Google Scholar
  33. Radach, R., Inhoff, A. W., Glover, L., & Vorstius, C. (2013). Contextual constraints and N + 2 preview effects in reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 619–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Rayner, K. (1975). The perceptual span and peripheral cues during reading. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 65–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Reilly, R., & Radach, R. (2012). The dynamics of reading in non-Roman writing systems. Reading and Writing, 25, 935–950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009a). Time course and task dependence of emotion effects in word processing. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2009b). Emotions in word and face processing: Early and late cortical responses. Brain and Cognition, 69, 538–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Schroyens, W., Vitu, F., Brysbaert, M., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1999). Eye movement control during reading: Foveal load and parafoveal processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 1021–1046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schupp, H. T., Junghöfer, M., Weike, A. I., & Hamm, A. O. (2003). Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychological Science, 14, 7–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., & Sereno, S. C. (2012). Emotion words affect eye fixations during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 783–792.Google Scholar
  41. Trauer, S. M., Kotz, S. A., & Müller, M. M. (2015). Emotional words facilitate lexical but not early visual processing. BMC Neuroscience, 16, 89.  https://doi.org/10.1186/s12868-015-0225-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Vasilev, M. R., & Angele, B. (2017). Parafoveal preview effects from word N + 1 and word N + 2 during reading: A critical review and Bayesian meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1147-x.Google Scholar
  43. Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 585–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wickham, H. (2009). Ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Yan, M. (2015). Visually complex foveal words increase the amount of parafoveal information acquired. Vision Research, 111, 91–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Yan, M., Kliegl, R., Shu, H., Pan, J., & Zhou, X. (2010). Parafoveal load of word N + 1 modulates preprocessing effectiveness of word N + 2 in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1669–1676.Google Scholar
  47. Yan, M., Risse, S., Zhou, X., & Kliegl, R. (2012). Preboundary duration modulates semantic preview benefit for word n + 1 and n + 2 in Chinese reading. Reading and Writing, 25, 1093–1111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Yan, M., & Sommer, W. (2015). Parafoveal-on-foveal effects of emotional word semantics in reading Chinese sentences: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1237–1243.Google Scholar
  49. Yan, M., Zhou, W., Shu, H., & Kliegl, R. (2015). Perceptual span depends on font size during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 209–219.Google Scholar
  50. Yang, J., Rayner, K., Li, N., & Wang, S. (2012). Is preview benefit from word n + 2 a common effect in reading Chinese? Evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 25, 1079–1091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature B.V. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MacauTaipaMacau SAR
  2. 2.Institut für PsychologieHumboldt-Universität zu BerlinBerlinGermany

Personalised recommendations