Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 32, Issue 1, pp 217–233 | Cite as

Writing difficulties in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment

  • Olivia AfonsoEmail author
  • Carlos J. Álvarez
  • Carmen Martínez
  • Fernando Cuetos
Article

Abstract

The present study addresses the scope of the writing difficulties observed in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Patients with AD, patients with MCI and healthy controls performed a written picture-naming task and a direct copy transcoding task in which phonology-to-orthography (P–O) consistency was manipulated. Written latencies, inter-letter durations, mean word pen pressure and number of errors were measured. Results revealed that there was a significant difference between the control group and both groups of patients in written latencies and number of errors. However, in inter-letter interval or mean pen pressure there were significant differences only between the group with AD and the other groups. P–O consistency and the type of task produced similar effects in the three groups of participants, suggesting a general deficit affecting the ability to access/generate orthographic representations. The relevance of these results for understanding the spelling impairment in AD and MCI are discussed.

Keywords

Handwriting Alzheimer’s disease Mild cognitive impairment Spelling-to-dictation Copying 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by Grant PSI2015-64174P and Grant PSI2013-47959-P from the Spanish Government. Olivia Afonso was the recipient of a research Grant AP2007-00065 from the “Canarian Foundation Doctor Manuel Morales”. The authors would like to thank Margaret Gillon Dowens for her helpful comments on earlier drafts. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Olivia Afonso, Department of Psychology, Health and Professional Development, Oxford Brookes University, Gipsy Lane, OX3 0BP.

References

  1. Afonso, O., & Álvarez, C. J. (2011). Phonological effects in handwriting production: Evidence from the implicit priming paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1474–1483.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024515.Google Scholar
  2. Afonso, O., Álvarez, C. J., & Kandel, S. (2015a). Effects of grapheme-to-phoneme probability on writing durations. Memory & Cognition, 43, 579–592.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0489-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2015b). Spelling impairments in Spanish dyslexic adults. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00466.Google Scholar
  4. Afonso, O., Suárez-Coalla, P., González-Martín, N., & Cuetos, F. (2017). The impact of word frequency on peripheral processes during handwriting: A matter of age. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1275713.Google Scholar
  5. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.).  https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349
  6. Beauvois, M. F., & Derouesné, J. (1981). Lexical or orthographic agraphia. Brain, 104, 21–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bonin, P., Méot, A., Lagarrigue, A., & Roux, S. (2015). Written object naming, spelling-to-dictation, and immediate copying: Different tasks, different pathways? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 1268–1294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bonin, P., Roux, S., Barry, C., & Canell, L. (2012). Evidence for a limited-cascading account of written word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1741–1758.Google Scholar
  9. Bub, D., & Kertesz, A. (1982). Deep agraphia. Brain and Language, 17, 146–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Caramazza, A. (1988). Some aspects of language processing revealed through the analysis of acquired aphasia: The lexical system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 11, 395–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Caramazza, A., Miceli, G., Villa, G., & Romani, C. (1987). The role of the graphemic buffer in spelling: Evidence from a case of acquired dysgraphia. Cognition, 26, 59–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chertkow, H., & Bub, D. (1990). Semantic memory loss in dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Brain, 113, 397–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cortese, M. J., Balota, D. A., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., & Buckner, R. L. (2003). Spelling via semantics and phonology: Exploring the effects of age, Alzheimer’s disease, and primary semantic impairment. Neuropsychologia, 41, 942–957.Google Scholar
  14. Croisile, B., Brabant, M., Carmol, T., Lepage, Y., Aimard, G., & Trillet, M. (1996). Comparison between oral and written spelling in Alzheimer disease. Brain and Language, 54, 361–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cuetos, F. (1991). Psicología de la Escritura: Diagnóstico y Tratamiento de los Trastornos de Escritura. Madrid: Escuela Española, S.A.Google Scholar
  16. Cuetos, F., Martínez, T., Martínez, C., Izura, C., & Ellis, A. W. (2003). Lexical processing in Spanish patients with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 549–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cuetos, F., Samartino, T., & Ellis, A. W. (2012). Age acquisition norms from elderly Spanish people: Characteristics and the prediction of word recognition performance in Alzheimer’s disease. Psicológica, 33, 59–76.Google Scholar
  18. Cummings, J. L., Benson, F., Hill, M. A., & Read, S. (1985). Aphasia in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neurology, 35, 394–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Davis, C. J., & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: A program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 665–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Delattre, M., Bonin, P., & Barry, C. (2006). Written spelling to dictation: Sound-to-spelling regularity affects both writing latencies and durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1330–1340.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1330.Google Scholar
  21. Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information processing rate and amount: Implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Forbes, K. E., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, A. (2004). The evolution of dysgraphia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Research Bulletin, 63, 19–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Graham, N. L., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (1997). Progressive dysgraphia: Co-occurrence of central and peripheral impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 975–1005.  https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Graham, N. L., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). The impact of semantic memory impairment on spelling: Evidence from semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia, 8, 143–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Guinet, E., & Kandel, S. (2010). Ductus: A software package for the study of handwriting production. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 326–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic memory consistently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neurospsychologia, 33, 441–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Houghton, G., & Zorzi, M. (2003). Normal and impaired spelling in a connectionist dual-route architecture. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 115–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hughes, J. C., Graham, N., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. (1997). Dysgraphia in mild dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Neuropsychologia, 35, 533–545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kandel, S., & Perret, C. (2015). How does the interaction between spelling and motor processes build up during writing acquisition? Cognition, 136, 325–336.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. LaBarge, E., Smith, D. S., Dick, L., & Storandt, M. (1992). Agraphia in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 49, 1151–1156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lambert, E., Alamargot, D., Larocque, D., & Caporossi, G. (2011). Dynamics of the spelling process during a copy task: Effects of regularity and frequency. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(3), 141–150.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., Graham, N., Dawson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2003). Homogeneity and heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of 55 cases. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 126, 2350–2362.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Luzzatti, C., Laiacona, M., & Agazzi, D. (2003). Multiple patterns of writing disorders in dementia of the Alzheimer type and their evolution. Neuropsychologia, 41, 759–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS–ADRDA work group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Miceli, G., Benvegnù, B., Capasso, R., & Caramazza, A. (1997). The independence of phonological and orthographic forms: Evidence from aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 35–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Nebes, R. D., Brady, C. B., & Huff, F. J. (1989). Automatic and attentional mechanisms of semantic priming in Alzheimer’s Disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 219–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Onofri, E., Mercuri, M., Donato, G., & Ricci, S. (2015). Cognitive fluctuations in connection to dysgraphia: A comparison of Alzheimer’s disease with dementia Lewy bodies. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 10, 625–633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (1985). Surface dyslexia. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 58, 1985–1992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Price, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). The effects of surface detail on object categorization and naming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 797–827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Purcell, J. J., Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., & Rapp, B. (2011). Examining the central and peripheral processes of written word production through meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 239.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rapcsak, S. Z., Arthur, S. A., Bliklen, D. A., & Rubens, A. B. (1989). Lexical agraphia in Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Neurology, 46, 65–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J., Martínez, C., Pérez-Carbajal, A. J., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Neural correlates of spelling difficulties in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropshycologia, 65, 12–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rossion, B., & Pourtois, G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object set: The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 33, 217–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Roux, J. S., McKeeff, T. J., Grosjacques, G., Afonso, O., & Kandel, S. (2013). The interaction between central and peripheral processes in handwriting production. Cognition, 127, 235–241.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, M. A., Carreiras, M., & Cuetos, F. (2000). LEXESP: Informatizado del Español. Barcelona: Ediciones de la Universitat de Barcelona.Google Scholar
  48. Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Shallice, T. (1981). Phonological agraphia and the lexical route in writing. Brain, 104, 413–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Silveri, M. C., Corda, F., & Di Nardo, M. (2007). Central and peripheral aspects of writing disorders in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 179–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2001). The spelling process. In B. Rapp (Ed.), The Handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind (pp. 263–289). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  52. Tainturier, M. J., & Rapp, B. (2003). Is a single graphemic buffer used in reading and spelling? Aphasiology, 17, 537–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: Issues for a psychomotor theory. Human Movement Science, 10(2–3), 165–191.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-9457(91)90003-G.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Werner, P., Rosenblum, S., Bar-On, G., Heinik, J., & Korczyn, A. (2006). Handwriting process variables discriminating mild Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. The Journals of Gerontology, 61, 228–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zhang, Q., & Feng, C. (2017). The interaction between central and peripheral processing in Chinese handwritten production: Evidence from the effect of lexicality and radical complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media B.V., part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Oxford Brookes UniversityOxfordUK
  2. 2.Universidad de La LagunaTenerifeSpain
  3. 3.Hospital de CabueñesGijónSpain
  4. 4.Universidad de OviedoOviedoSpain

Personalised recommendations