Advertisement

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research

, Volume 48, Issue 5, pp 987–1004 | Cite as

Causal Information Over Facial Expression: Modulation of Facial Expression Processing by Congruency and Causal Factor of the Linguistic Cues in 5-Year-Old Japanese Children

  • Yun-hee ParkEmail author
  • Shoji Itakura
Article
  • 69 Downloads

Abstract

It is unknown whether linguistic cues influence preschoolers’ recognition of facial expression when the emotion of the face is incongruent with the linguistic cues and what type of linguistic cue is influential in the modulation of facial expression. In a priming task, we presented 5-year-old children three types of linguistic information conveying happy or sad emotion (simple label, label with rich causal explanation of emotion, and label with poor causal explanation of emotion) prior to the presentation of a happy or a sad face. We asked participants to recognize emotion of facial expression and examined whether they follow linguistic information than the facial expression when the emotions of linguistic information and face were incongruent. As a result, children who were presented the label with rich causal explanation judged emotion of face according to the linguistic information, whereas children who were presented the simple label or the label with poor causal explanation followed the emotions of facial expression. This result indicated that children’s autonomic reliance on situational cues depends on the emotional causality and the concreteness of emotional state conveyed by the linguistic cues. This finding contributes to our understanding of the face inferiority effect on children’s developing notion of emotions.

Keywords

Preschoolers Linguistic cue Facial expression Face inferiority effect 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a Grant to S.I. from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (25245067, 25240020).

References

  1. Aguert, M., Laval, V., Le Bigot, L., & Bernicot, J. (2010). Understanding expressive speech acts: the role of prosody and situational context in French-speaking 5- to 9-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1629–1641.  https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0078).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Banerjee, M. (1997). Hidden emotions: Preschoolers’ knowledge of appearance-reality and emotion display rules. Social Cognition, 15, 107–132.  https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.1997.15.2.107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2007). Language as context for the perception of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 327–332.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bullock, M., & Russell, J. A. (1986). Concepts of emotion in developmental psychology. In C. E. Izard & P. B. Read (Eds.), Measuring emotions in infants and children (Vol. 2, pp. 203–237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  6. Carroll, J. M., & Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 205–218.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.2.205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Diéguez-Risco, T., Aguado, L., Albert, J., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2013). Faces in context: Modulation of expression processing by situational information. Social Neuroscience, 8, 601–620.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.834842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Diéguez-Risco, T., Aguado, L., Albert, J., & Hinojosa, J. A. (2015). Judging emotional congruency: Explicit attention to situational context modulates processing of facial expressions of emotion. Biological Psychology, 112, 27–38.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.09.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dunn, J., Brown, J., & Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children’s later understanding of others’ emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27, 448–455.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Egami, S. (2010). Preschool children’s understanding of others’ emotions: Focusing on a difference in emotions. Bulletin of the Faculty of Education Ehime University, 57, 27–32.Google Scholar
  11. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Ellsworth, P. (2013). Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and an integration of findings. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  12. Ekman, P., & O’Sullivan, M. (1988). The role of context in interpreting facial expression: Comment on Russell and Fehr (1987). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 86–88.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.1.86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–454.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ferré, P., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2014). Affective priming in a lexical decision task: Is there an effect of words’ concreteness? Psicológica, 35, 117–138.Google Scholar
  15. Ford, J. A., & Milosky, L. M. (2003). Inferring emotional reactions in social situations differences in children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 21–30.  https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/002).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Foster, K. I., & Foster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 35, 116–124.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gardner, D., Harris, P. L., Ohmoto, M., & Hamazaki, T. (1988). Japanese children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 203–218.  https://doi.org/10.1177/016502548801100204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gnepp, J. (1983). Children’s social sensitivity: Inferring emotions from conflicting cues. Developmental Psychology, 19(6), 805–814.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.19.6.805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gnepp, J., Klayman, J., & Trabasso, T. (1982). A hierarchy of information sources for inferring emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33(1), 111–123.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(82)90009-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gnepp, J., McKee, E., & Domanic, J. A. (1987). Children’s use of situational information to infer emotion: Understanding emotionally equivocal situations. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 114–123.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gross, D., & Harris, P. L. (1988). False beliefs about emotion: Children’s understanding of misleading emotional displays. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 475–488.  https://doi.org/10.1177/016502548801100406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Halberstadt, J. B., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2001). Effects of emotion concepts on perceptual memory for emotional expressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 587–598.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harris, P. L. (2008). Children’s understanding of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 70–83). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., & Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development, 57, 895–909.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hayashi, M. (2004). Discourse within a sentence: An exploration of postpositions in Japanese as an interactional resource. Language in Society, 33(3), 343–376.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404504043027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hillier, A., & Allinson, L. (2002). Beyond expectations. Autism, understanding embarrassment, and the relationship with theory of mind. Autism. The International Journal of Research and Practice, 6, 299–314.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361302006003007.Google Scholar
  27. Hoffner, C., & Badzinski, D. M. (1989). Children’s integration of facial and situational cues to emotion. Child development.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130986.Google Scholar
  28. Josephs, I. E. (1994). Display rule behavior and understanding in preschool children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 301–326.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Joshi, M. S., & MacLean, M. (1994). Indian and English children’s understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Development, 65, 1372–1384.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00822.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kikuchi, T. (2004). Development of young children’s understanding of their own facial expressions. The Japanese Journal of DevelopmentalPsychology, 15, 207–216.  https://doi.org/10.5926/jjep1953.54.1_90.Google Scholar
  31. Kim, H., Somerville, L. H., Johnstone, T., Polis, S., Alexander, A. L., Shin, L. M., et al. (2004). Contextual modulation of amygdala responsivity to surprised faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1730–1745.  https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929042947865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Klauer, K. C., & Musch, J. (2003). Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 7–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Putting feelings into words affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 8, 421–428.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lindquist, K. A., & Gendron, M. (2013). What’s in a word? Language constructs emotion perception. Emotion Review, 5(1), 66–71.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912451351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lupyan, G. (2012). Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label-feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054.Google Scholar
  36. Morton, J. B., & Trehub, S. E. (2001). Children’s understanding of emotion in speech. Child Development, 72, 834–843.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127–152.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620344000022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Reichenbach, L., & Masters, J. C. (1983). Children’s use of expressive and contextual cues in judgments of emotion. Child Development.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1129903.Google Scholar
  39. Ridgeway, D., Waters, E., & Kuczaj, S. A. (1985). Acquisition of emotion-descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabulary norms for ages 18 months to 6 years. Developmental Psychology, 21, 901–908.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.5.901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Roberson, D., & Davidoff, J. (2000). The categorical perception of colors and facial expressions: The effect of verbal interference. Memory and Cognition, 28, 977–986.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Russell, J. A., & Widen, S. C. (2002). A label superiority effect in children’s categorization of facial expressions. Social Development, 11, 30–52.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schwarz, K. A., Wieser, M. J., Gerdes, A. B., Mühlberger, A., & Pauli, P. (2013). Why are you looking like that? How the context influences evaluation and processing of human faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 438–445.  https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spackman, M. P., Fujiki, M., & Brinton, B. (2006). Understanding emotions in context: the effects of language impairment on children’s ability to infer emotional reactions. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 173–188.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820500224091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Theurel, A., Witt, A., Malsert, J., Lejeune, F., Fiorentini, C., Barisnikov, K., et al. (2016). The integration of visual context information in facial emotion recognition in 5- to 15-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 252–271.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Trauner, D. A., Ballantyne, A., Chase, C., & Tallal, P. (1993). Comprehension and expression of affect in language-impaired children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22, 445–452.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01074346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Trope, Y. (1986). Identification and inferential processes in dispositional attribution. Psychological Review, 93, 239–257.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wang, Z., Lü, W., Zhang, H., & Surina, A. (2014). Free-labeling facial expressions and emotional situations in children aged 3–7 years: Developmental trajectory and a face inferiority effect. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(6), 487–498.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414531462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2004). The relative power of an emotion’s facial expression, label, and behavioral consequence to evoke preschoolers’ knowledge of its cause. Cognitive Development, 19, 111–125.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2010). Differentiation in preschooler’s categories of emotion. Emotion, 10, 651.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wiggers, M., & van Lieshout, C. F. (1985). Development of recognition of emotions: Children’s reliance on situational and facial expressive cues. Developmental Psychology, 21(2), 338–349.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.2.338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Xu, F., & Kushnir, T. (2013). Infants are rational constructivist learners. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 28–32.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412469396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Graduate School of LettersKyoto UniversityKyotoJapan

Personalised recommendations