Advertisement

A Note on the Voice Mismatch Asymmetry in Ellipsis

  • Charles CliftonJr.Email author
  • Ming Xiang
  • Lyn Frazier
Article
  • 28 Downloads

Abstract

Theories of ellipsis differ in the identity condition claimed to hold between an antecedent and an elided constituent. A syntactic identity condition leads to the prediction that syntactic mismatches between an antecedent and elided constituent should give rise to a penalty, and that penalty should be greater than in corresponding examples without ellipsis. Further, if syntactic mismatches are ungrammatical, violating the syntactic identity condition, then in effect they are speech errors and would be expected to be rated higher when a passive clause antecedes an active elided VP than vice versa because people misremember passives as actives more often the reverse. A written acceptability judgment study crossed the voice of the antecedent clause (active/passive), the voice of the ellipsis clause (active/passive) and ellipsis/non-ellipsis in the final clause. Results indicate a syntactic mismatch lowers acceptability in examples with elided VPs but not examples with overt VPs, as predicted by theories with a syntactic identity condition. Passive-active mismatches were rated better than active–passive ones, especially with ellipsis, as predicted by a speech error/repair approach to mismatches. This result eliminates any concern that the appearance of a voice asymmetry might only be due to some incompatibility between VP ellipsis and passive voice.

Keywords

Sentence comprehension Ellipsis VPE Sentence acceptability 

Notes

References

  1. Arregui, A., Clifton, C., Jr., Frazier, L., & Moulton, K. (2006). Processing elided VP s with flawed antecedents. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 232–246.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Christensen, R. H. B. (2018). Cumulative link models for ordinal regression with the R package Ordinal. From https://cran.r-roject.org/web/packages/ordinal/vignettes/clm_article.pdf.. Retrieved January 4, 2019.
  3. Christensen, R. H. B., & Brockhoff, P. B. (2013). Analysis of sensory ratings data with cumulative link models. Journal de la Societe Francaise de Statisique & Revue de Statistique Appliquee, 154(3), 58–79.Google Scholar
  4. Frazier, L., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2011). Quantifiers undone: Reversing predictable speech errors in comprehension. Language, 87(1), 158–171.  https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2011.0024.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Frazier, L., & Clifton, C., Jr. (2015). Without his shirt off he saved the child from almost drowning: Interpreting an uncertain input. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(6), 635–647.  https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.995109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Frazier, L., & Duff, J. (To appear). Repair or accommodation? Split antecedent ellipsis and the limits of repair. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics.Google Scholar
  7. Grant, M., Clifton, C., Jr., & Frazier, L. (2012). The role of non-actuality implicatures in processing elided constituents. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 326–343.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.09.003.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Hardt, D. (1990). A corpus based survey of VP ellipsis. Ms., University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
  9. Hardt, D. (1992). VP ellipsis and semantic identity. In Proceedings of the Stuttgart ellipsis workshop, Stuttgart.Google Scholar
  10. Hardt, D. 1993. Verb phrase ellipsis: Form meaning, and processing. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
  11. Hardt, D. (1999). Dynamic interpretation of verb phrase ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy, 22, 187–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hendriks, P. (2004). Coherence relations, ellipsis, and contrastive topics. Journal of Semantics, 21(2), 133–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Johnson, K. (2001). What VP ellipsis can do, and what it can’t, but not why. In M. Baltin & C. Collins (Eds.), The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory (pp. 439–479). Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kehler, A. (2002). Coherence, reference and the theory of grammar. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publication Inc.Google Scholar
  15. Kehler, A. (2015). On QUD-based licensing of strict and sloppy ambiguities. Presented at Semantics and Linguistic Theory 25 (SALT-25), May 15–17, 2015.Google Scholar
  16. Kertz, L. (2008). Focus structure and acceptability in verb phrase ellipsis. In Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics (Vol. 27, pp. 283–291). Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  17. Kertz, L. (2013). Verb phrase ellipsis: The view from information structure. Language, 89(3), 390–428.  https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2013.0051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kim, C., & Runner, J. T. (2018). The division of labor in explanations of verb phrase ellipsis. Linguistics and Philosophy, 41, 41–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Liddell, T. M., & Kruschke, J. K. (2018). Analyzing ordinal data with metric models: What could possibly go wrong? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 328–348.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.08.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Mehler, J. (1963). Some effects of grammatical transformations on the recall of English sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 346–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Merchant, J. (2008). An asymmetry in voice mismatches in VP-ellipsis and pseudogapping. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 169–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Merchant, J. (2013). Voice and ellipsis. Linguistic Inquiry, 44, 77–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Miller P. (2011). The choice between verbal anaphors in discourse. In I. Hendrickx, S. Lalitha Devi, A. Branco, & R. Mitkov (Eds.) Anaphora processing and applications. DAARC 2011. Lecture Notes in Computer Science (Vol. 7099). Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
  24. Miller, P., Hemforth, B., Flambard, G., & Amsili, P. (2019). Missing antecedents found. Poster presented at the 93rd Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, New York, January 3–6.Google Scholar
  25. Poppels, T., & Kehler, A. (2017). Overcoming the identity crisis: novel evidence for a referential theory of verb phrase ellipsis. In Proceedings of the Chicago linguistics society, May 25–27.Google Scholar
  26. Poppels, T., & Kehler, A. (2018). Asymmetries in voice-mismatch VP-ellipsis. Paper presented at the 54th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  27. R Development Core Team. (2017). A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
  28. Sag, I. (1976). Deletion and logical form. PhD dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  29. Tanenhaus, M. K., & Carlson, G. N. (1990). Comprehension of deep and surface verbphrase anaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5, 257–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Thoms, G. (2015). Syntactic identity, parallelism and accommodated antecedents. Lingua, 166, 172–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Webber, B. (1978). A formal approach to discourse anaphora. PhD thesis, Harvard UniversityGoogle Scholar
  32. Williams, E. (1977). Discourse and logical form. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 101–139.Google Scholar
  33. Xiang, M., & Klafka, J. (2018). Memory retrieval in comprehension is sensitive to production alternatives. Poster presented at the 31st Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Davis, CA.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesUniversity of Massachusetts AmherstAmherstUSA
  2. 2.Department of LinguisticsUniversity of ChicagoChicagoUSA
  3. 3.Department of LinguisticsUniversity of Massachusetts AmherstAmherstUSA

Personalised recommendations