Advertisement

Emotion Attribution in Nonverbal Vocal Communication Directed to Preterm Infants

  • Manuela Filippa
  • Maria Grazia Monaci
  • Didier Grandjean
Original Paper

Abstract

The first weeks of life for preterm babies are critical for bonding and attachment. Early vocal contact, as a family based intervention, engages mothers to speak and to sing to their preterm infants in the incubators. This study tested the emotional and smiling content of the mother’s speaking and singing voice in a context of early contact with her newborn preterm infant in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU), and it examines which acoustic parameters are associated with emotion attributed to the mother’s voice. The main hypothesis is that the emotional content of maternal speech and song, when directed to their preterm infants placed in incubators, is modulated by the infants’ behaviors. Thirty-two blocks of vocal extracts in the presence and absence of preterm infants’ displays and in the 5 s preceding each display were presented to 31 adult naïve listeners who were asked to rate the degree of emotion and smile in the mother’s voice. The present results show that when infants open the eyes or smile, the maternal voice is perceived as more emotional and more smiling than it is in the absence of any facial display. This effect is particularly evident in the maternal speech. The maternal voice is rated as more smiling in the presence of the infant’s smile than when preterm infants opened the eyes. The main acoustical features of the infant-directed voice—mean pitch and perceived sound pressure level—are positively associated with smiling attribution while only the mean pitch is associated with emotional intensity. These findings extends prior evidences by showing that maternal ID speech and songs, even in at-risk conditions such as prematurity, are not only related to preterm infant behavior, but also bear emotional content. Early face-to-face interactions between mothers and preterm infants need to be encouraged in the NICU and investigated, especially neural correlates, to assess the impact of emotional infant-directed voices on preterm infants’ brain development.

Keywords

Preterm infant-directed speech Preterm infant-directed songs Emotion perception Early vocal contact Neonatal intensive care unit 

References

  1. Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2008). On privileging the role of gaze in infant social cognition. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 59–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aubergé, V., & Cathiard, M. (2003). Can we hear the prosody of smile? Speech Communication, 40, 87–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 89–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2003). Praat-A system for doing phonetics by computer [Computer Software]. The Netherlands: Institute of Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  6. Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 1584–1595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Corbeil, M., Trehub, S. E., & Peretz, I. (2013). Speech vs. singing: Infants choose happier sounds. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Delavenne, A., Gratier, M., Devouche, E., & Apter, G. (2008). Phrasing and fragmented time in “pathological” mother-infant vocal interaction. Musicae Scientiae, 12(Suppl. 1), 47–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eckerman, C. O., Hsu, H. C., Molitor, A., Leung, E. H., & Goldstein, R. F. (1999). Infant arousal in an en-face exchange with a new partner: Effects of prematurity and perinatal biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 35, 282–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eckerman, C. O., Oehler, J. M., Medvin, M. B., & Hannan, T. E. (1994). Premature newborns as social partners before term age. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 55–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ekman, P., Davidson, R. J., & Friesen, W. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Emde, R. N., McCartney, R. D., & Harmon, R. J. (1971). Neonatal smiling in REM states, IV: Premature study. Child Development, 42, 1657–1661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (Kangaroo Care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 274–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 279–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20, 104–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language, 16, 477–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Filippa, M., Devouche, E., Arioni, C., Imberty, M., & Gratier, M. (2013). Live maternal speech and singing have beneficial effects on hospitalized preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica, 102, 1017–1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Filippa, M., Gratier, M., Devouche, E., & Grandjean, D. (2018). Changes in infant-directed speech and song are related to preterm infant facial expression in the neonatal intensive care unit. Interaction Studies.Google Scholar
  19. Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P., et al. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, i-149.Google Scholar
  20. Kawakami, K., Takai-Kawakami, K., Kawakami, F., Tomonaga, M., Suzuki, M., & Shimizu, Y. (2008). Roots of smile: A preterm neonates’ study. Infant Behavior & Development, 31(3), 518–522.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.03.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lester, B. M., Hoffman, J., & Brazelton, T. B. (1985). The rhythmic structure of mother-infant interaction in term and preterm infants. Child Development, 56, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Merrill, J., Sammler, D., Bangert, M., Goldhahn, D., Lohmann, G., Turner, R., et al. (2012). Perception of words and pitch patterns in song and speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Messinger, D., Dondi, M., Christina Nelson-Goens, G., Beghi, A., Fogel, A., & Simion, F. (2002). How sleeping neonates smile. Developmental Science, 5(1), 48–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Messinger, D. S., Fogel, A., & Dickson, K. L. (2001). All smiles are positive, but some smiles are more positive than others. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 642–653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Nadel, J., & Butterworth, G. (Eds.). (1999). Imitation in infancy (Vol. 16). Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Nakagawa, S., & Schielzeth, H. (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 4, 133–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nakata, T., & Trehub, S. E. (2004). Infants’ responsiveness to maternal speech and singing. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(4), 455–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Sauter, D. A., & Eimer, M. (2010). Rapid detection of emotion from human vocalizations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 474–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 2408–2412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of emotion: A review of research paradigms. Speech Communication, 40, 227–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Shi, R., Werker, J. F., & Harado, M. (1997, April). Maternal facial expressions provide infants with information about vowels: A cross-language study. Poster presented at the biannual meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
  32. Singh, L., Morgan, J. L., & Best, C. T. (2002). Infants’ listening preferences: Baby talk or happy talk? Infancy, 3, 365–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Smith, N. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2008). Infant-directed speech is modulated by infant feedback. Infancy, 13, 410–420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18, 727–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stroup, W. W. (2016). Generalized linear mixed models: Modern concepts, methods and applications. Boca Raton: CRC Press.Google Scholar
  36. Tang, S. K., Chan, D. W., & Chan, K. C. (1997). Prediction of sound-pressure level in an occupied enclosure. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 2990–2993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tartter, V. C. (1980). Happy talk: Perceptual and acoustic effects of smiling on speech. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 27, 24–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Trainor, L. J., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotion? Psychological Science, 11, 188–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wolff, P. H. (1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy: New proposals for investigation. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  40. Wood, A., Martin, J., & Niedenthal, P. (2017). Towards a social functional account of laughter: Acoustic features convey reward, affiliation, and dominance. PLoS ONE, 12, e0183811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J., & Keller, H. (2012). A cross-cultural comparison of the development of the social smile: A longitudinal study of maternal and infant imitation in 6-and 12-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 335–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Faculty of Psychology and Educational SciencesUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
  2. 2.Swiss Center of Affective SciencesUniversity of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
  3. 3.Development and Growth Division, Child and Adolescent DepartmentUniversity Hospital of GenevaGenevaSwitzerland
  4. 4.Faculty of PsychologyUniversity of Valle d’AostaAostaItaly

Personalised recommendations