Advertisement

The Effects of Machine Balance and Free-Spins Features on Machine Preference and Bet Amounts

  • Lydia A. ChapmanEmail author
  • Maree J. Hunt
  • Lorance F. Taylor
  • Anne C. Macaskill
Original Paper
  • 45 Downloads

Abstract

Across two experiments, the current study investigated whether the balance displayed on slot-machine screens affects gamblers’ decision making in a manner similar to the “house-money effect” observed in other gambling modes. The balance indicates the total amount the gambler has available to wager from. We manipulated initial slot-machine balance within a simulated slot-machine task. Participants gambled on each of two machines and then were able to switch between the two allowing us to assess the effect of machine balance on both preference and bet amounts. Experiment 1 also manipulated which machine incorporated a free-spins feature. In both experiments participants preferred to wager on, and made larger bets on, machines with higher balances. Experiment 1 replicated previous findings that people prefer to gamble on machines offering free spins. Together, these two experiments identify balance available as an additional slot-machine feature that contributes to slot-machine gambling behavior.

Keywords

Slot machines EGMs Pokie machines Free spins House-money effect Gambling 

Notes

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Victoria University of Wellington’s School of Psychology Human Ethics Committee under delegated authority of Victoria University of Wellington’s Human Ethics Committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

References

  1. Arkes, H. R., Joyner, C. A., Pezzo, M. V., Nash, J. G., Siegel-Jacobs, K., & Stone, E. (1994). The psychology of windfall gains. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 59, 331–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Dixon, M. J., Collins, K., Harrigan, K. A., Graydon, C., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). Using sound to unmask losses disguised as wins in multiline slot machines. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 183–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dixon, M. J., Harrigan, K. A., Sandhu, R., Collins, K., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2010). Losses disguised as wins in modern multi-line video slot machines. Addiction, 105, 1819–1824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dixon, M., MacLin, O., & Daugherty, D. (2006). An evaluation of response allocations to concurrently available slot machine simulations. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 232–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Giroux, I., & Ladouceur, R. (2006). The effect of near wins on the choice of a video lottery terminal. Gambling Research, 18, 69–75.Google Scholar
  6. Griffiths, M. (1993). Fruit machine gambling: The importance of structural characteristics. Journal of Gambling Studies, 9, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hsu, Y., & Chow, E. H. (2013). The house money effect on investment risk taking: Evidence from Taiwan. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 21, 1102–1115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Kahneman, D., & Tvsersky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Kurucz, G., & Körmendi, A. (2012). Can we perceive near miss? A empirical study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28, 105–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Landon, J., Palmer du Preez, K., Page, A., Bellringer, M., Roberts, A., & Abbott, M. (2018). Electronic gaming machine characteristics: It’s the little things that count. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6, 251–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1184–1188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Levy, J. S. (1992). An introduction to prospect theory. Political Psychology, 13(2), 171–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Livingstone, C., Woolley, R., Zazryn, T., Bakacs, L., & Shami, R. (2008). The relevance and role of gaming machine games and game features of the play of problem gamblers. South Australia: Independent Gambling Authority.Google Scholar
  14. Millhouse, H., & Delfabbro, P. (2008). Investigating preferences for gaming machine features in problem and non-problem gamblers using a consumer choice methodology. Gambling Research: Journal of the National Association for Gambling Studies (Australia), 20, 37–49.Google Scholar
  15. Taylor, L., Macaskill, A., & Hunt, M. (2017). Realistic free-spins features increase preference for slot machines. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33, 555–577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Thaler, R. H., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management Science, 36, 643–660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Walker, M. B., & Phil, D. (1992). Irrational thinking among slot machine players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 8, 245–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.C/- School of PsychologyVictoria University of WellingtonWellingtonNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations